Huberman Lab - How to Learn Skills Faster

Welcome to the Huberman Lab Podcast,

where we discuss science

and science-based tools for everyday life.

I’m Andrew Huberman,

and I’m a professor of neurobiology and ophthalmology

at Stanford School of Medicine.

This podcast is separate from my teaching

and research roles at Stanford.

It is, however, part of my desire and effort

to bring zero cost to consumer information

about science and science-related tools

to the general public.

In keeping with that theme,

I’d like to thank the sponsors of today’s podcast.

Our first sponsor is Athletic Greens.

Athletic Greens is an all-in-one

vitamin mineral probiotic drink.

I’ve been taking Athletic Greens since 2012,

so I’m delighted that they’re sponsoring the podcast.

The reason I started taking Athletic Greens

and the reason I still take Athletic Greens

once or twice a day

is that it helps me cover

all of my basic nutritional needs.

It makes up for any deficiencies that I might have.

In addition, it has probiotics,

which are vital for microbiome health.

I’ve done a couple of episodes now

on the so-called gut microbiome

and the ways in which the microbiome interacts

with your immune system, with your brain to regulate mood,

and essentially with every biological system

relevant to health throughout your brain and body.

With Athletic Greens, I get the vitamins I need,

the minerals I need,

and the probiotics to support my microbiome.

If you’d like to try Athletic Greens,

you can go to athleticgreens.com slash Huberman

and claim a special offer.

They’ll give you five free travel packs

plus a year supply of vitamin D3 K2.

There are a ton of data now showing that vitamin D3

is essential for various aspects

of our brain and body health.

Even if we’re getting a lot of sunshine,

many of us are still deficient in vitamin D3.

And K2 is also important

because it regulates things like cardiovascular function,

calcium in the body, and so on.

Again, go to athleticgreens.com slash Huberman

to claim the special offer of the five free travel packs

and the year supply of vitamin D3 K2.

Today’s episode is also brought to us by Element.

Element is an electrolyte drink

that has everything you need and nothing you don’t.

That means the exact ratios of electrolytes are an element,

and those are sodium, magnesium, and potassium,

but it has no sugar.

I’ve talked many times before on this podcast

about the key role of hydration and electrolytes

for nerve cell function, neuron function,

as well as the function of all the cells

and all the tissues and organ systems of the body.

If we have sodium, magnesium, and potassium

present in the proper ratios,

all of those cells function properly

and all our bodily systems can be optimized.

If the electrolytes are not present,

and if hydration is low,

we simply can’t think as well as we would otherwise,

our mood is off, hormone systems go off,

our ability to get into physical action,

to engage in endurance and strength,

and all sorts of other things is diminished.

So with Element, you can make sure

that you’re staying on top of your hydration

and that you’re getting the proper ratios of electrolytes.

If you’d like to try Element, you can go to drinkelement,

that’s lmnt.com slash Huberman,

and you’ll get a free Element sample pack

with your purchase.

They’re all delicious.

So again, if you want to try Element,

you can go to elementlmnt.com slash Huberman.

Today’s episode is also brought to us by Thesis.

Thesis makes what are called nootropics,

which means smart drugs.

Now, to be honest, I am not a fan of the term nootropics.

I don’t believe in smart drugs in the sense that

I don’t believe that there’s any one substance

or collection of substances that can make us smarter.

I do believe based on science, however,

that there are particular neural circuits

and brain functions that allow us to be more focused,

more alert, access creativity, be more motivated, et cetera.

That’s just the way that the brain works,

different neural circuits for different brain states.

Thesis understands this.

And as far as I know, they’re the first nootropics company

to create targeted nootropics for specific outcomes.

I’ve been using Thesis for more than six months now,

and I can confidently say that their nootropics

have been a total game changer.

My go-to formula is the clarity formula,

or sometimes I’ll use their energy formula before training.

To get your own personalized nootropic starter kit,

go online to takethesis.com slash Huberman,

take a three-minute quiz,

and Thesis will send you four different formulas

to try in your first month.

That’s takethesis.com slash Huberman,

and use the code Huberman at checkout

for 10% off your first order.

I’m pleased to announce that the Huberman Lab Podcast

is now partnered with Momentus Supplements.

We partnered with Momentus for several important reasons.

First of all, they ship internationally

because we know that many of you are located

outside of the United States.

Second of all, and perhaps most important,

the quality of their supplements is second to none,

both in terms of purity and precision

of the amounts of the ingredients.

Third, we’ve really emphasized supplements

that are single ingredient supplements,

and that are supplied in dosages

that allow you to build a supplementation protocol

that’s optimized for cost,

that’s optimized for effectiveness,

and that you can add things and remove things

from your protocol in a way

that’s really systematic and scientific.

If you’d like to see the supplements

that we partner with Momentus on,

you can go to livemomentus.com slash Huberman.

There you’ll see those supplements,

and just keep in mind that we are constantly expanding

the library of supplements available through Momentus

on a regular basis.

Again, that’s livemomentus.com slash Huberman.

This month on the Huberman Lab Podcast,

we’re talking all about physical performance.

So that means athletic performance, recreational exercise,

weightlifting, running, swimming, yoga,

skills and skill learning.

Today, we’re going to talk about

and focus on skill learning.

We are going to focus on how to learn skills more quickly,

in particular motor skills.

This will also translate to things like musical skills

and playing instruments,

but we’re mainly going to focus on physical movements

of the body that extend beyond the hands,

like just playing the piano,

or the fingers like playing the guitar.

But everything we’re going to talk about

will also serve the formation and the consolidation

and the performance of other types of skills.

So if you’re interested in how to perform better,

whether or not it’s dance or yoga,

or even something that’s just very repetitive

like running or swimming,

this podcast episode is for you.

We’re going to go deep into the science of skill learning,

and we are going to talk about very specific protocols

that the science points to and has verified

allow you to learn more quickly,

to embed that learning so that you remember it,

and to be able to build up skills more quickly

than you would otherwise.

We are also going to touch on a few things

that I get asked about a lot,

but fortunately recently,

I’ve had the time to go deep into the literature,

extract the data for you,

and that’s mental visualization.

How does visualizing a particular skill or practice

serve the learning and or the consolidation

of that practice?

It turns out there are some absolutely striking protocols

that one can use,

striking meaning they allow you to learn faster,

and they allow you to remember how to do things

more quickly and better

than if you were not doing this mental rehearsal,

but the pattern of mental rehearsal,

and when you do that mental rehearsal

turns out to be vitally important.

So I’m excited for today’s episode.

We’re going to share a lot of information with you,

and there are going to be a lot of very simple takeaways.

So let’s get started.

Before we get into the topic of skill learning

and tools for accelerating skill learning,

I want to briefly revisit the topic of temperature,

which was covered in the last episode

and just highlight a few things

and clear up some misunderstandings.

So last episode talked about these incredible data

from my colleague, Craig Heller’s lab at Stanford.

He’s in the department of biology

showing that cooling the palms in particular ways

and at particular times can allow athletes

or just recreational exercisers

to do more pull-ups, dips, bench presses per unit time,

to run further, to cycle further,

and to feel better doing it.

There really are incredible data

that are anchored in the biology of the vascular system,

the blood supply and how it’s involved in cooling us.

Many of you, dozens of you in fact,

said, wait a second, you gave us a protocol in this episode

which says that we should cool our palms periodically

throughout exercise in order to be able to do more work.

But on the episode before that

on growth hormone and thyroid hormone,

you said that heating up the body

is good for release of growth hormone.

And I just want to clarify that both things are true.

These are two separate protocols.

You should always warm up before you exercise.

That warmup will not increase your body temperature

or the muscle temperature to the point

where it’s going to diminish your work capacity,

that it’s going to harm your performance.

The cooling of the palms,

which is really just a route to cool your core

in an efficient way, the most efficient way in fact,

is about improving performance.

Heating up the body with exercise

and focusing on heat increases

or using sauna for heat increases

is geared toward growth hormone release,

which is a separate matter.

So you can do both of these protocols

but you would want to do them at separate times.

So just to make this very concrete

before I move on to today’s topic,

if you’re interested in doing more work,

being able to do more sets and reps per unit time

and feel better doing it or to run further

or to cycle further, then cooling the palms periodically

as I described in the previous episode

is going to be the way to go.

If you’re interested in getting growth hormone release,

well then hot sauna, and I offered some other tools

if you don’t have a sauna in the episode

on growth hormone and thyroid hormone

is going to be the way to go.

Okay, so those are separate protocols.

You can include them in your fitness regime

and your training regime,

but you do want to do them at separate times.

And as a last point about this,

I also mentioned that caffeine can either help

or hinder performance

depending on whether or not you’re caffeine adapted

because of the ways that caffeine impacts body temperature

and all sorts of things like vasodilation and constriction.

It’s very simple.

If you enjoy caffeine before your workouts

and you’re accustomed to caffeine,

meaning you drink it three or five times or more a week,

100 to 300 milligrams is a typical daily dose of caffeine.

Some of you are ingesting more, some less.

If you do that regularly,

well then it’s going to be just fine

to ingest caffeine before you train.

It’s not going to impact your body temperature

and your vasodilation or constriction

in ways that will hinder you.

However, if you’re not a regular caffeine user

and you’re thinking, oh, I’m going to drink a cup of coffee

and get this huge performance enhancing effect,

well, that’s not going to happen.

Chances are it’s going to lead to increases

in body temperature and changes in the way

that blood flow is happening in your body

and in particular on these palmar surfaces and in your face

that is going to likely diminish performance.

So if you enjoy caffeine and you’re accustomed to it,

so-called caffeine adapted, enjoy it before your training.

If you regularly, excuse me,

if you do not regularly use caffeine,

then you probably do not want to view caffeine

as a performance enhancing tool.

And while we’re on the topic of tools,

and because this is a month on athletic performance

and exercise and physical skill learning,

I want to offer an additional tool

that I’ve certainly found useful,

which is how to relieve the so-called side stitch

or side cramp when running or swimming.

This actually relates to respiration

and to the nervous system.

And it is not a cramp.

If you’ve ever been out running

and you felt like you had a pain on your side,

that pain could be any number of things,

but that what feels like cramping of your side

is actually due to what’s called collateralization

of the phrenic nerve, which is a lot harder to say

than a side cramp or a side stitch.

But here’s the situation.

You have a set of nerves,

which is called the phrenic nerve, P-H-R-E-N-I-C,

the phrenic nerve, which extends down

from your brainstem, essentially, this region,

to your diaphragm to control your breathing.

It has a collateral, meaning it has a branch,

just like the branch on a tree,

that innervates your liver.

And if you are not breathing deeply enough,

what can happen is you can get

what’s called sometimes a referenced pain.

Referenced pain is probably going to be familiar

to any of you who have ever read

about how to recognize heart attack.

You know, people who have heart attacks

will sometimes have pain on one side of their body,

the left arm.

Sometimes people that have pain in a part of their back

will suddenly also get pain in their shoulder

or part of their face.

This has to do with the fact

that many of our nerves branch,

meaning they’re collateralized

to different organs and areas of the body.

And the way those nerves are woven together,

it’s often the case that if we disrupt the pattern

of firing of electrical activity

in one of those nerve branches

that the other ones are affected too.

The side stitch, the pain in your side,

is often because of the contractions of the diaphragm

because of the way you’re breathing

while you’re exercising, running or swimming or biking.

And as a consequence, you feel pain in your side,

but that’s not a cramp.

The way to relieve it is very simple.

You do the physiological sigh that I’ve talked about

in previous episodes of the podcast and elsewhere,

which is a double inhale through the nose,

very deep, and then a long exhale.

And you might want to repeat that two or three times.

Typically, that will relieve the side stitch

because of the way that it changes the firing patterns

of the phrenic nerve.

So the side stitch is annoying, it’s painful.

Sometimes when you think we’re dehydrated

and you might be dehydrated,

but oftentimes it’s just that we’re breathing in a way

that causes some referenced pain of the liver.

We call it a side stitch or a side cramp,

and you can relieve it very easily

through the double inhale, long exhale.

That pattern done two or three times.

Often you can continue to engage in the exercise

while you do the double inhale, exhale,

and it will just relieve itself that way.

So give it a try if you experienced the side stitch.

Some people I know are also doing the double inhale,

long exhale during long continuous bouts of exercise.

I actually do this when I run.

We have decent data,

although these are still unpublished data

that can engage a kind of regular cadence

of heart rate variability.

So there are a number of reasons

why this physiological side can be useful,

but it certainly can be useful for relieving the side stitch

or so-called side cramp.

Let’s talk about the acquisition of new skills.

These could be skills such as a golf swing

or a tennis swing, or you’re shooting free throws,

or you’re learning to dance,

or you’re learning an instrument.

I’m mainly going to focus on athletic performance.

There are basically two types of skills,

open loop and closed loop.

Open loop skills are skills

where you perform some sort of motor action,

and then you wait and you get immediate feedback

as to whether or not it was done correctly or not.

A good example would be throwing darts at a dart board.

So if you throw the dart,

you get feedback about whether or not you hit the bullseye,

you’re off the dart board,

or you’re some other location on the dart board.

That’s open loop.

Closed loop would be something that’s more continuous.

So let’s say you’re a runner

and you’re starting to do some speed work and some sprints,

and you’re running and you can kind of feel

whether or not you’re running correctly

or maybe you even have a coach

and they’re correcting your stride

or you’re trying to do some sort of skill

like a hopscotch skill,

which maybe you’re doing the ladder work

where you’re stepping between designated spaces

on the ground.

That’s closed loop because as you go,

you can adjust your behavior

and you can adjust the distance of your steps,

or you can adjust your speed,

or you can adjust your posture.

And you are able to essentially do more practice

per unit time,

but you’re getting feedback on a moment to moment basis.

Okay, so you have open loop and closed loop.

And just to make this very, very clear,

open loop would be practicing your tennis serve.

So let’s say that you set a target

on the other side of the net,

you throw the ball up and you hit the ball, it goes over,

that’s open loop.

You’ll know whether or not you were in the court,

you were on the location you wanted to hit

or close to it or not.

That’s open loop.

Closed loop would be if you’re in a regular case,

so maybe you’re learning a swim stroke,

or maybe you’re trying to learn a particular rhythm

on the drum.

So maybe you’re trying to learn a particular beat.

I’m not very musical,

so I’m not going to embarrass myself

by giving an example of this,

although later I will,

where you’re trying to get a particular rhythm down.

And if you’re not getting it,

you can adjust in real time

and try and catch up or slow down or speed up, et cetera.

Okay, so hopefully you’ll understand open loop

and closed loop.

You should always know before you try and learn a skill,

whether or not it’s open loop or closed loop.

And I’ll return to why that’s important shortly.

But if you want to learn something,

ask is it open loop or closed loop?

There are essentially three components of any skill

that involves motor movement.

And those are sensory perception,

actually perceiving what you are doing

and what’s happening around you.

So what you see, what you hear,

sometimes you’re paying attention

to what you’re doing specifically,

like the trajectory of your arm

or how you’re moving your feet if you’re learning to dance.

Sometimes you’re more focused

on something that’s happening outside of you,

like you’re listening for something in music

or you’re paying attention

to the way your partner is moving, et cetera.

So there’s sensory input.

Then there are the actual movements.

Okay, so there are the movements of your limbs and body.

And then there’s something called proprioception.

And proprioception is often discussed

as kind of a sixth sense of knowing where your limbs are

in relation to your body.

So proprioception is vitally important.

If I reach down and pick up this pen and pick it up,

I’m not thinking about where the pen in my hand

is relative to my body,

but proprioceptively I’m aware of it

at kind of a sixth sense deeper subconscious level.

I can also make myself aware of where my limbs are.

And typically when we learn,

we are placing more focus on proprioception

than we do ordinarily.

So if I get up from this chair

and I happen to walk out of the room,

I don’t think about where my feet are landing

relative to one another.

But if my leg had fallen asleep

because I had been leaning on one of the nerves of my leg

or something like that,

and my leg feels all tingly or numb,

I and you, if this were to happen to you,

would immediately notice a shift in gait.

It would feel strange.

I’d have to pay attention to how I’m stepping.

And the reason is I’m not getting

any proprioceptive feedback.

Now, skill learning has a lot of other dimensions too,

but those are the main ones that we’re going to focus on.

So just to remind you,

it’s you need to know open loop or closed loop,

and you need to know whether or not, excuse me,

you need to know that there’s sensory perception,

what you’re paying attention to,

movements themselves, and proprioception.

And there’s one other important thing that you need to know,

which is that movement of any kind is generated

from one, two, or three sources within your nervous system,

within your brain and body.

These are central pattern generators,

which are sometimes called CSPGs, excuse me, CPGSs.

CSPGs are something entirely different in biology.

CPGSs, this just goes to show that I have a module.

CSPGs are chondroitin sulfate proteoglycans.

They have nothing to do with this topic.

CPGSs are central pattern generators,

or CPGs they’re sometimes called.

These CPGs are in your spinal cord, mine and yours,

different ones, and they generate repetitive movements.

So if you’re walking, if you’re running, if you’re cycling,

if you’re breathing, which presumably you are,

and you’re doing that in a regular rhythmic cadence,

central pattern generators are controlling that movement.

After you learn how to walk, run, swim, cycle,

do anything really, much of the work is handed off

to these central pattern generators.

And there were experiments that were done

in the 60s, 70s, and 80s

that actually looked at decerebrate animals

and even decerebrate humans.

These are humans and animals that lack a cerebral cortex.

They lack much of the brain,

and yet they can engage in what’s called a fictive movement.

So it sounds like a kind of barbaric experiment.

I’m glad I wasn’t the one to have to do them,

but this is the stuff of neuroscience textbooks

that cats or dogs or mice

that have their neocortex removed,

put them on a treadmill, they’ll walk just fine.

And they will adjust their speed of walking just fine,

even though they basically lack

all their thinking, decision-making brain.

And it turns out humans that have,

unfortunately, massive strokes to their cortex

and lack any neocortex,

but preserve the central pattern generators,

will also walk just fine,

even though they lack any of the other stuff in the brain.

So these CPGs or CPGSs are amazing,

and they control a lot of our already learned behavior.

When you’re really good at something,

CPGs are controlling a lot of that behavior.

And that’s true also for a golf swing,

even if it’s not really repetitive.

Somebody who’s really good at golf is going to,

I guess you call it a tee, you put the ball on the tee.

I show my knowledge of golf.

I’ve only done mini golf, frankly,

but someday maybe I’ll learn how to golf,

but you set the golf ball down and swing,

set the golf ball down, swing.

Central pattern generators are going to handle

a lot of that.

If I were to go to the golf course,

Stanford has a beautiful golf course.

If I were to go out there, put a ball on the tee,

my central pattern generators would not be involved

in that at all.

The moment I get, you know, bringing the club back to swing,

it’s going to engage other things.

And the other things it’s going to engage,

because I don’t know that behavior now or then,

is upper motor neurons.

We have motor neurons in our cortex, in our neocortex,

that control deliberate action.

And those are the ones that you’re engaging

when you are learning.

Those are the ones that you have to pay attention

in order to engage.

And that’s what’s happening, for instance,

if I decide I’m going to reach down and pick up my pen,

which I rarely think about, but now I’m thinking about it,

and I’m going to do this in a very deliberate way.

I’m going to grab with these two fingers and lift.

My upper motor neurons are now involved, okay?

So upper motor neurons are very important

because a little bit later in the episode,

when we talk about how to use visualization

in order to accelerate skill learning,

it’s going to leverage these upper motor neurons

in very particular ways, okay?

So we have CPGs for rhythmic movement,

upper motor neurons for deliberate unlearned movements

or movements that we are in the process of learning.

And then we have what are called lower motor neurons.

Lower motor neurons are the ones in our spinal cord

that send little wires out to our muscles,

which actually cause the firing of those muscle fibers, okay?

So the way to think about this

is you’ve got upper motor neurons,

which talk to CPGs and to lower motor neurons.

So it’s really simple.

And now you know most everything there is to know

about the neural pathways controlling movement,

at least for sake of this discussion.

So anytime we learn something,

we have to decide what to place our sensory perception on,

meaning what are we going to focus on?

That’s critical.

If you’re listening to this

and you’re the type of person who likes taking notes,

this should be the second question you ask.

Remember, the first question is,

is it open loop or closed loop?

The second question should be,

what should I focus my attention on?

Auditory attention, visual attention, or proprioception.

Should I focus on where my limbs are relative to my body,

or should I focus on the outcome?

Okay, this is a critical distinction.

You can decide to learn how to do a golf swing

or learn how to shoot free throws

or learn how to dance tango

and decide that you are going to focus on

the movements of your partner or the positions of your feet.

Maybe you’re going to look at them.

Maybe you’re going to sense them.

You’re going to actually feel where they are.

Or maybe you’re going to sense the position

and posture of your body, which is more proprioceptive, okay?

So you have to allocate your attention,

and I’m going to tell you how to allocate your attention

best in order to learn faster.

So these are the sorts of decisions that you have to make.

Fortunately for you, you don’t have to think about

whether or not you’re going to use your upper motor neurons

and your lower motor neurons or not,

because if you don’t know how to do something,

you’re automatically going to engage

your upper motor neurons.

And if you do, then you’re not going to use

your upper motor neurons.

You’re mainly going to rely on central pattern generators.

You are always using your lower motor neurons to move muscle.

So we can really simplify things now.

I’ve given you a lot of information, but we can simplify it.

Basically, open loop or closed loop, that’s one question.

And what am I going to focus on?

And then your neurology will take care of the rest.

So now I want to talk about realistic expectations.

Somewhere in Hollywood, presumably,

it got embedded in somebody’s mind

that instant skill acquisition was possible,

that you could take a particular pill

or you could touch a particular object

or you could have a wand wave over you

and you would suddenly have a skill.

And so that is the result of Hollywood at all.

It doesn’t exist, at least not in reality.

And I love movies, but it simply doesn’t exist.

Then the self-help literature created another rule

called the 10,000 hours rule.

And frankly, that doesn’t really match the literature,

at least the scientific literature, either.

I like it because it implies that learning takes time,

which is more accurate than the Hollywood at all

instant skill acquisition rule, which isn’t really a rule.

It’s a myth.

But the 10,000 hours rule overlooks something crucial,

which is that it’s not about hours, it’s about repetitions.

Now, of course, there’s a relationship

between time and repetitions,

but there are some beautiful experiments

that point to the fact that by simple adjustment

of what you are focused on

as you attempt to learn a new skill,

you can adjust the number of repetitions that you do,

you adjust your motivation for learning,

and you can vastly accelerate learning.

Some of you may recognize this by its internet name,

which is not a scientific term,

which is the Super Mario effect.

There’s actually a quite good video on YouTube

describing the Super Mario effect.

I think it was a YouTuber who has,

I think a background in science,

and he did an interesting experiment.

And I’ll talk about his experiment first,

and then I will talk about the neurobiology

that supports the result that he got.

The Super Mario effect relates

to the game Super Mario Brothers,

but you’ll see why at the end.

But basically what they did was they had 50,000 subjects,

which is a enormous number of subjects,

learn a program, essentially taking words

from a computer program or the commands

for a computer program that were kind of clustered

in a column on the right.

So these are the sorts of things

that computer programmers will be familiar with,

but other people won’t.

And those commands are essentially,

they essentially translate to things like,

go forward, and then if it’s a right-hand turn in the maze,

then go right and continue

until you hit a choice point, et cetera.

So it’s a bunch of instructions,

but the job of the subjects in these experiments

were to organize those instructions in a particular way

that would allow a little cursor

to move through the maze successfully, okay?

So basically the goal was,

or at least what the subjects were told,

is that anyone can learn to computer program,

and if somebody can just organize the instructions

in the right way, then they can program

this little cursor to move through a maze, very simple.

And yet, if you don’t have any background

in computer programming, or even if you do,

it takes some skill.

You have to know what commands to give

in what particular order, and they made that very easy.

You could just assemble them in a list

over onto the right.

So people started doing this.

Now, there were two groups,

and one half of the subjects,

if they got it wrong,

meaning they entered a command and the cursor would move,

and it was the wrong command

for this little cursor to move through the maze,

they saw a signal jump up on their screen

that said, that did not work, please try again.

That’s it.

If they put in the wrong command

or it was in the wrong sequence,

it would say, that did not work, please try again.

And then the subjects would reorganize the instructions,

and then the little cursor would continue.

And if they got it wrong again,

it would say, that did not work, please try again, okay?

The other half of the subjects,

if they got something wrong,

were told, you just lost five points, please continue.

So that’s the only difference in the feedback that they got.

Now, I have to confess,

I would have predicted based on my knowledge

of dopamine circuitry and reward contingency

and epinephrine and stress and motivated learning,

and this other thing that we’ve been told

in many, many books on behavioral economics

and in the self-help literature,

which is that people will work much harder

to prevent losing something

than they will to gain something.

That you hear all the time.

And it turns out that that’s not at all what happened.

If they looked at the success rate of the subjects,

what they found was that the subjects that were told,

that did not work, please try again,

had a 68% success rate.

68% of them went on to successfully program

this cursor moving through the maze.

Whereas the ones that were told you lost five points

had a 52% success rate,

which is a significant difference.

But the source of the success

or the lack of success is really interesting.

The subjects that were told,

that did not work, please try again,

tried many, many more times per unit time.

In other words, they made more attempts

at programming this thing

to allow this cursor to move through the maze.

Whereas the people that were told you lost five points

gave up earlier or gave up entirely.

Okay, so let’s just step back from this

because to me, this was very surprising.

It violates a lot of things that I had heard

in the kind of popular culture or the self-help literature

that people will work much harder

to avoid losing something than they will to gain something.

And it didn’t really fit with what I understood

about reward contingencies and dopamine.

But it did fit well with another set of experiments

that I’m very familiar with

from the neuroscience literature.

And I’ll give you the punchline first.

And then we’re going to take what these data mean

and we’re going to talk about a learning protocol

that you can use that will allow you to learn skills faster

by willingly participating in more repetitions

of the skill learning.

Meaning you will want to do more repetitions

even if you’re getting it wrong some or most of the time.

So the experiment that I want to tell you about

is called the tube test.

And this is generally done in mice

although it’s sometimes been done in rats

and it has a lot of parallels to some things

that you’ve probably seen and experienced

even in human life, in regular life,

maybe even in your life.

So here’s the experiment.

You take two rats, you put them in a tube

or two mice, you put them in a tube

and mice and rats, they don’t like to share the same tube.

So what they’ll do is they’ll start pushing each other

back and forth, back and forth.

Sooner or later, one of the rats or mice

pushes the other one out.

The one that got pushed out is the loser.

The one that gets the tube is the winner.

Okay?

Now you take the winner, you give it a new competitor.

And what you find is that the mouse or rat

that won previously has a much higher

than chance probability of winning the second time.

In other words, winning before leads to winning again.

And the reverse is also true.

If you take the loser and you put that loser

in with another mouse, fresh mouse, new mouse,

the loser typically will lose

at much greater probability than chance.

And this is not related to differences in strength

or size or testosterone or any other of the things

that might leap to mind as explanations for this

because those were all controlled for.

Now, that result had been known about for decades,

but three years ago, there was a paper published

in the journal Science, phenomenal journal.

It’s one of the three apex journals

that examined the brain area that’s involved in this.

Turns out it’s a particular area of the frontal cortex

for those of you that want to know.

And they did a simple experiment

where the experimenters increased or decreased

the activity of this brain area in the prefrontal cortex,

a little sub-region of the prefrontal cortex.

And what they found is if they stimulated this brain area,

a mouse or rat, regardless of whether or not

it had been a winner or loser before,

became a winner every single time.

And they showed that if they blocked the activity

of this brain area, regardless of whether or not

the mouse or rat had been a winner or a loser,

it became a loser every single time.

And this translated to other scenarios,

other competitive scenarios where they’d put a bunch

of mice or rats in a kind of cool chamber.

They’d have a little heat lamp in the corner

and mice like heat.

And there was only enough space for one mouse

to be under the heat.

And the one that had won in the tube test

or that had the brain area stimulated

always got the nice warm spot.

Okay, so what is this magic brain area?

What is it doing?

Well, the reason I’m bringing this up today

and the reason I’m bringing it up on the heels

of the Super Mario effect is that stimulation

of this brain area had a very simple

and very important effect,

which was it led to more forward steps,

more repetitions, more effort,

but not in terms of sheer might and will,

not digging deeper, just more repetitions per unit time.

And the losers had fewer repetitions per unit time.

So the Super Mario effect, this online experiment

and the tube test, which has been done by various labs

and repeated again and again,

point to a simple but very important rule,

which is neither the 10,000 hours rule

nor the magic wand Hollywood version of learning,

but rather the neurobiological explanation

for learning a skill is you want to perform

as many repetitions per unit time as you possibly can,

at least when you’re first trying to learn a skill.

I want to repeat that.

You want to perform as many repetitions as you possibly can,

at least when you’re first trying to learn a skill.

Now that might sound like a duh, it’s just more reps,

but it’s not so obvious.

There’s no reason why more repetitions

should necessarily lead to faster learning

because you could also say, well, more repetitions,

you can make more errors.

And those errors would lead to poor performance

like misstepping a number of times.

And in these cases, there’s very little feedback, right?

It’s not like every time the rat pushes forward

or moves back that it is sensing, oh, I’m winning,

I’m losing, I’m winning, I’m losing on a micro level.

It probably does that as it starts to push the other one out

the rat or mouse probably thinks, I’m winning.

And as it’s backing up, it probably thinks I’m losing.

As you play the game, the Super Mario game,

you are told, nope, that didn’t work.

Nope, that didn’t work, please try again.

But the important thing is that the winners

are always generating more repetitions per unit time.

It’s just a repeat of performance, repeat of performance,

even if there are errors.

And that points to something vitally important,

which is reps are important,

but making error reps is also important.

In fact, it might be the most important factor.

So let’s talk about errors

and why those solve the problem of what to focus on.

Because as I said earlier,

if you want to learn something,

you need to know if it’s open loop or closed loop,

and you need to know what to focus on,

where to place your perception.

And that seems like a tough task,

but errors will tell you exactly what to focus on.

So let’s talk about errors and why you can leverage errors

to accelerate skill learning.

Okay, so we’ve established that performing

the maximum number of repetitions per training session

is going to be advantageous.

And that might seem obvious,

but there’s a shadowy side to that, which is,

well, why would I want to just repeat the same thing

over and over again if I’m getting it wrong 90% of the time?

And the reason is that the errors

actually cue your nervous system to two things.

One, to error correction,

and the other is it opens the door

or the window for neuroplasticity.

Neuroplasticity is the brain and nervous system’s ability

to change in response to experience,

essentially to custom modify itself

in order to perform anything better.

We did an entire month on neuroplasticity,

and I talked a little bit about errors

and why they’re important.

Now we’re going to make this very concrete

and operationalize it, make it very actionable.

There was a paper that was published in 2021

from Norman et al.

This is a very important paper.

It was published in the journal Neuron,

which is a cell press journal, excellent journal.

The title of the paper gives it away essentially,

which is, post-error recruitment

of frontal sensory cortical projections promotes attention.

Now, what that says is that when you make an error,

it causes an activation of the brain areas

that anchor your attention.

Remember, we need perception, attention,

which they’re essentially the same thing.

We need proprioception,

and we need the upper and lower motor neurons

to communicate in the proper ways.

And this vital question is what to pay attention to.

Errors tell your nervous system

that something needs to change.

So if you are performing a task or a skill,

like you’re learning how to dance

and you’re stepping on the other person’s toes

or you’re fumbling or you’re not getting it right,

those errors are opening the possibility for plasticity.

If you walk away at that point,

you’ve made the exact wrong choice, okay?

Unless the errors are somehow hazardous to your health

or somebody else’s wellbeing,

you want to continue to engage at a high repetition rate.

That’s really where the learning is possible.

Without errors, the brain is not in a position

to change itself.

Errors actually cue the frontal cortex networks,

what we call top-down processing,

and the neuromodulators,

things like dopamine and acetylcholine and epinephrine,

that will allow for plasticity.

So while the Super Mario experiment, the maze experiment,

was only focused on generating errors,

telling people that wasn’t right, please try again,

or that wasn’t right, you lost five points,

the key distinction is that the errors themselves

cued people to the fact

that they needed to change something.

So if you’re trying to learn a new skill

and you’re screwing up and you’re making mistakes,

the more mistakes you make,

the more plastic your brain becomes,

such that when you get it right,

that correct pattern will be rewarded and consolidated.

And you can trust that it will

because the performance of something correctly

is associated with the release

of this neuromodulator dopamine.

Dopamine is involved in craving and motivation.

It’s involved in a lot of things,

but it’s also involved in learning.

We will do an entire episode on dopamine and learning,

but because some of you are probably wondering,

this does not mean that just increasing your dopamine levels

before learning will allow you to learn faster.

In fact, increasing your dopamine levels

before learning using pharmacology

will actually reduce what’s called the signal to noise.

It will make these increases in dopamine

that pop up in your brain

that suddenly make you realize, ah, I got that one right.

It will make those smaller

relative to the background levels of dopamine, okay?

You want a big spike in dopamine

when you perform a motor pattern correctly,

and you want to make lots of errors,

many, many repetitions of errors

in order to get to that correct performance.

Now, if you’re like most people,

you’re going to do this in a way that’s somewhat random,

meaning, let’s say it’s a tennis serve.

I can’t play tennis.

I think I’ve probably played tennis twice.

So if I throw the ball up in the air and hit it,

I’m going to get it wrong and probably hit the net.

Then I’m going to hit the net.

Then I’ll probably go too long.

Then I’ll probably go over the fence.

At some point, I like to think I’ll get it correct.

The dopamine signal for that is going to be quite big.

And I’ll think, okay, what did I do there?

I actually don’t know.

I wasn’t paying attention.

What I was paying attention to is whether or not

the ball went to the correct location

on the opposite side of the net.

Remember, it’s an open loop move.

So I don’t actually know what I did correctly,

but your nervous system will take care of that

provided I, in this case,

complete more and more and more repetitions.

Now, if I were to just elevate my basal level of dopamine

by taking, I don’t know,

1500 milligrams of L-tyrosine or something,

that would be bad because the increase in dopamine

would actually be much lower, right?

We would say the delta is smaller.

The signal to noise is smaller

if my overall levels of dopamine are very, very high.

So I’m actually going to learn less well.

So for skill learning, motor skill learning,

increasing your dopamine levels prior is not a good idea.

It might help with motivation to get to the learning,

but it’s not going to improve the plasticity process itself

and it’s likely to hinder it, okay?

So that’s very important.

So these errors cue the brain that something was wrong

and they open up the possibility for plasticity.

It’s what’s sometimes called the framing effect.

It frames what’s important.

And so I think this is a shift.

We’ve heard about, you know, growth mindset,

which is the incredible discovery and theory and practice

of my colleague, Carol Dweck at Stanford.

This is distinct from that, right?

This isn’t about motivation to learn.

This is about how you actually learn.

So the key is designate a particular block of time

that you are going to perform repetitions.

So maybe that’s 30 minutes, maybe that’s an hour.

Work for time and then try and perform

the maximum number of repetitions that you can do safely

for you and others per unit time.

That’s going to be the best way

to approach learning for most sessions.

I will talk about other things that one can do,

but making errors is key.

And this isn’t a motivational speech.

I’m not saying, oh, go make errors.

Errors are good for you.

You have to fail in order to win.

No, you have to fail in order to open up

the possibility of plasticity,

but you have to fail many times within the same session.

And those failures will cue your attention

to the appropriate sensory events.

Now, sometimes we’re working with a coach.

And so this is a shout out to all the coaches.

Thank you for doing what you do.

However, there needs to be,

at least what the scientific literature say,

there needs to be a period of each training session

whereby the athlete or the person of any kind

can simply pay attention to their errors

without their attention being cued to something else.

A really well-trained coach will say,

oh, you know, your elbow is swinging too high

or you’re not gripping the racket

in the appropriate way, et cetera.

They can see things that the practitioner can’t see.

And of course that’s vitally important.

But the practitioner also needs

to use this error recognition signal.

They need to basically focus on something

and the errors are going to tell them what to focus on.

So put simply, there needs to be a period of time

in which it’s just repetition

after repetition, after repetition.

I think many people, including coaches,

are afraid that bad habits will get ingrained.

And while indeed that’s possible,

it’s very important that these errors occur

in order to cue the attentional systems

and to open the door for plasticity.

So if I’m told, look, you know, I’m standing a little wide,

I need to tighten up my stance a little bit, great.

But then I need to generate many repetitions

from that tightened stance, okay?

So if I’m constantly being cued from the outside

about what I’m doing incorrectly,

that’s not going to be as efficient, okay?

So for some people,

these learning sessions might be 10 minutes.

For some people, it might be an hour.

Whatever you can allocate,

because your lifestyles will vary

and whether or not you’re a professional athlete,

et cetera, will vary.

You want to get the maximum number of repetitions in

and you want to make errors.

That’s allowing for plasticity.

So science points to the fact

that there’s a particular sequencing of learning sessions

that will allow you to learn faster

and to retain the skill learning.

And it involves doing exactly as I just described,

which is getting as many repetitions as you can

in the learning session,

paying attention to the errors that you make,

and then the rewards that will be generated,

again, these are neurochemical rewards,

from the successful performance of a movement

or the approximate successful performance.

So maybe you get the golf swing better, but not perfect,

but that’s still going to be rewarded

with this neurochemical mechanism.

And then after the session,

you need to do something very specific, which is nothing.

That’s right.

There are beautiful data describing neurons

in our hippocampus, this area of our brain involved

in the consolidation of new memories.

Those data point to the fact that in sleep,

there’s a replay of the sequence of neurons

that were involved in certain behaviors the previous day,

and sometimes the previous day before that.

However, there are also data that show

that after a skill learning session,

any kind of motor movement,

provided you’re not bringing in

a lot more additional new sensory stimuli,

there’s a replay of the motor sequence

that you performed correctly,

and there’s an elimination of the motor sequences

that you performed incorrectly,

and they are run backward in time.

Okay, so to be very clear about this,

if I were to learn a new skill or navigate a new city,

or let’s just stay with the motor skill,

let’s say the free throw or a golf swing or a tennis serve,

dance move, novice, so I’m still going to make

a lot of errors, don’t get it perfectly,

but maybe I get a little bit better

or I perform it correctly three times out of 1,000,

that sounds like something I might do,

and there I’m probably being generous to myself.

After I finished the training session,

if I do nothing, I’m not focused on some additional learning,

I’m not bringing a lot of sensory information in,

if I just sit there and close my eyes

for five to 10 minutes, even one minute,

the brain starts to replay the motor sequence

corresponding to the correct pattern of movement,

but it plays that sequence backward.

Now, why it plays it backward, we don’t know.

If I were to wait until sleep or regardless,

when I sleep later that night,

the sequence will be replayed forwards

in the proper sequence.

Immediately afterward, it’s played backward

for reasons that are still unclear,

but the replay of that sequence backwards

appears to be important for the consolidation

of the skill learning.

Now, this is important because many people

are finishing their jujitsu class

or they’re finishing their yoga class

or they’re finishing their dance class

or they’re finishing some skill learning,

and then they’re immediately devoting their attention

to something else.

You hear a lot about visualization

and we are going to talk about visualization,

but in the kind of obsession with the idea

that we can learn things just sitting there

with our eyes closed without having to perform a movement,

we’ve overlooked something perhaps even more important

or at least equally important,

which is after skill learning,

after putting effort into something,

sitting quietly with the eyes closed

for one to five to 10 minutes

allows the brain to replay the sequence

in a way that appears important

for the more rapid consolidation

of the motor sequence of the pattern

and to accelerated learning.

If you’d like to learn more about this,

this is not work that I was involved in,

I want to be very clear.

There’s an excellent paper that covers this and much more

for those of you that really want to dive deep on this,

and we will dive deeper in a moment.

This is a review that was published in the journal Neuron,

excellent journal.

Many of the papers that I’m referring to

were covered in this review,

which is titled,

Neuroplasticity Subserving Motor Skill Learning

by Dayan, D-A-Y-A-N,

I hope I’m not butchering the pronunciation,

and Cohen by Leonard Cohen,

but not the Leonard Cohen most of us are familiar with,

the musician Leonard Cohen.

Dayan and Cohen,

Neuroplasticity Subserving Motor Skill Learning.

And this was published in 2011,

but there’ve been a number of updates

and the literature that I’ve described

in other portions of today’s episode

come from the more recent literature,

such as the more recent 2021 paper, okay?

So you have this basic learning session

and then a period of time afterwards

in which the brain can rehearse what it just did.

We hear so much about mental rehearsal,

and we always think about mental rehearsal

as the thing you do before you train or instead of training,

but this is rehearsal that’s done afterward

where the brain is just automatically scripting

through the sequence,

and for some reason that’s still not clear

as to why this would be the case, it runs backward.

Then in sleep, it runs forwards,

and certainly, absolutely, sleep and quality sleep

of the appropriate duration, et cetera,

is going to be important for learning of all kinds,

including skill learning.

We did an entire four episodes on sleep

and how to get better at sleeping.

Those are the episodes back in January,

episodes essentially one, two, three, and four,

and maybe even episode five, I don’t recall,

but you can go there to find out

all about how to get better at sleeping.

Now, there are other training sessions involved, right?

I’m not going to learn the perfect golf swing

or the tennis serve or how to dance in one session,

and I doubt you will either.

So the question is when to come back

and what to do when you come back to the training session.

Now, first of all,

this principle of errors cuing attention

and opening the opportunity for plasticity,

that’s never going to change.

That’s going to be true for somebody who is hyperskilled,

who has mastery or even virtuosity in a given skill, right?

Remember, when you’re unskilled at something,

uncertainty is very high.

As you become more skilled, certainty goes up, right?

Then eventually you achieve levels of mastery

where certainty is very, very high

about your ability to perform,

yours, certainty, and that of other people.

And then there’s this fourth category of virtuosity

where somebody, maybe you,

invites uncertainty back into the practice

because only with that uncertainty

can you express your full range of abilities,

which you aren’t even aware of

until uncertainty comes into the picture, right?

I happen to have the great privilege

of being friends with Laird Hamilton,

the big wave surfer who’s phenomenal.

I don’t surf.

I certainly don’t surf with Laird,

but he and another guy that he surfs with, Luka Padua,

these guys are, they’re virtuosos at surfing.

They don’t just want the wave that they can master,

they want uncertainty.

They’re at the point in their practice

where when uncertainty shows up,

like a wave that’s either so big

or is moving in a particular way

that it brings an element of uncertainty for them

about what they’re going to do,

they recognize that as the opportunity

to perform better than they would otherwise, okay?

So they’re actually trying to eliminate uncertainty.

At the beginning of learning any skill

and as we approach from uncertain to skilled to mastery,

we want to reduce uncertainty.

And that’s really what the nervous system is doing.

It’s trying to eliminate errors

and hone in on the correct trajectories.

If you perform a lot of repetitions

and then you use a period immediately after,

we don’t really have a name for this.

Maybe someone will come up with it

and put it in the comment section if you’re on YouTube,

if you’re watching this on YouTube,

a name for this post-learning kind of idle time

for the brain.

The brain isn’t idle at all.

It’s actually scripting all these things in reverse

that allow for deeper learning and more quick learning.

But if we fill that time with other things,

if we are focused on our phones

or we’re focused on learning something else,

we’re focusing on our performance,

that’s not going to serve us well.

At least it’s not going to serve the skill learning well.

So please, if you’re interested

in more rapid skill learning,

try introducing these sessions.

They can be quite powerful.

And then on subsequent sessions,

presumably after a night’s sleep

or maybe you’re doing two sessions a day,

although two sessions a day is going to be a lot

for most people, unless you’re a professional

or a high-level athlete.

The subsequent sessions are where you get to express

the gains of the previous session, right?

Where you get to perform well,

presumably more often, even if it’s just subtle,

sometimes there’ll be a decrease in performance,

but most often you’re going to perform better

on subsequent and subsequent training sessions.

And there is the opportunity to devote attention

in very specific ways, right?

Not just let the errors inform you

where to place your attention,

but rather to direct your perception

to particular elements of the movement

in order to accelerate learning further, okay?

So to be very clear,

because I know many of you are interested

in concrete protocols,

it’s not just that you would only let errors

cue your attention on the first session.

You might do that for one session or five sessions.

It’s going to depend.

But once you’re familiar with something

and you’re performing it well every once in a while,

you’re accomplishing it better every once in a while,

then you can start to cue your attention

in very deliberate ways.

And the question therefore becomes

what to cue your attention to.

And the good news is it doesn’t matter.

There is a beautiful set of experiments that have been done

looking at sequences of keys being played on a piano.

This is work that was published just a couple of years ago.

There are actually several papers now

that are focused on this.

One of them was published in 2018.

This is from Claudia Lappe and colleagues, L-A-P-P-E.

She’s done some really nice work,

which talks about the influence of pitch feedback

on learning of motor timing and sequencing.

And this was done with piano,

but it carries over to athletic performance as well.

So I’m going to describe this study to you.

But before I describe it,

what is so interesting about this study

that I want you to know about

is that it turns out it doesn’t matter so much

what you pay attention to during the learning sequence,

provided it’s something related to the motor behavior

that you’re performing, right?

That seems incredible, right?

I’m not good at a tennis serve.

So if I’ve done, you know,

let’s say a thousand repetitions of the tennis serve,

maybe I got it right three to 10 times.

Now I’m being even more generous with myself.

And I do this post-training session

where I let my brain idle and I get some good sleep

and then I come back

and now I start generating errors again,

presumably or hopefully fewer errors,

but I decide I’m going to cue my attention

to something very specific,

like maybe how tightly I’m holding the racket,

or maybe it’s my stance,

or maybe it’s whether or not I rotate my right shoulder in

as I hit the ball across,

and I’m making this up again, I don’t play tennis.

Turns out that as long as it’s the same thing

throughout the session, learning is accelerated.

And I’ll explain why this makes sense in a moment,

but just to be really clear,

you can and one should use your powers of attention

to direct your attention

to particular aspects of a motor movement

once you’re familiar with the general theme of the movement,

but what you pay attention to exactly is not important.

What’s important is that you pay attention

to one specific thing.

So what Claudia Lape in college showed was that

if people are trying to learn a sequence of keys

on the piano, there are multiple forms of feedback.

There are error signals,

if for instance, they hear a piece of music

and then they’re told to press the keys

in a particular sequence,

and the noise that comes out,

the sound that comes out of the piano

does not sound like the song they just heard, right?

So instead of, and here forgive me

because I’m neither musical nor can I sing,

but instead of da-da-da-da-da-da,

they hear that da-da-da-da-da-da-da,

and then instead when they play,

or for me it would sound something like da-da-da-da-da-da,

it wouldn’t sound right, okay?

It wouldn’t sound right

because I likely got the sequence wrong

or I was pressing too hard on the keys

or too lightly on the keys, et cetera.

What they showed was if they just instruct people

about the correct sequence to press on the keys,

it actually doesn’t matter what sound comes back

provided it’s the correct sound or it’s the same sound.

All right, so here’s the experiment.

They had people press on these keys

and it was a typical piano

and it generated the particular sequence of sounds

that would be generated by pressing the keys on the piano.

Or they modified the keyboard in this case or piano

such that when people pressed on the keys,

a random tone, different tones were played

each time they pressed on the keys.

So it sounded crazy, it sounded like noise,

but the motor sequence was the same.

Or they had a single tone

that was played every time they pressed a key

and the job or the task of the subject

was just to press the sequence,

press the keys in the proper sequence.

So instead of dun-dun-dun-dun-dun-dun-dun,

it was just dun-dun-dun-dun-dun-dun-dun.

Instead of da-da-da-da-da-da-da,

it’s da-da-da-da-da-da-da.

It’s even hard for me to say it in an even tone,

but you get the idea.

So a singular tone, just think a doorbell being rung

with each press of the key would be really annoying, okay?

But it turns out that the rate to motor learning

was the same whether or not they were getting feedback

that was accurate to the keys of the piano

or whether or not it was a constant tone.

Performance was terrible

and the rates of learning were terrible

if they were getting random tones back.

So what this means is that learning to play the piano,

at least at these early stages,

is really just about generating the motor commands.

It’s not about paying attention to the sound

that’s coming out of the piano.

And this makes sense because when we are beginners,

we are trying to focus our attention

on the things that we can control.

And if you think about this, if you conceptualize this,

pressing the keys on the piano

and paying attention to the sounds

that are coming out are two things.

So what this means is that as you get deeper

and deeper into a practice,

focusing purely on the motor execution can be beneficial.

Now, this is going to be harder to do

with open loop type things where you’re getting feedback.

I guess a good example of open loop

would be the attempt at a backflip, right?

If you get it wrong, you will immediately know.

If you get it right, you’ll immediately know.

Please don’t go out and try and do a backflip

on the solid ground or even on a trampoline

if you don’t know what you’re doing

because very likely you’ll get it wrong

and you’ll get injured.

But if it’s something that is closed loop

where you can repeat again and again and again and again,

that is advantageous

because you can perform many, many repetitions

and you can start to focus

or learn to focus your attention

just on the pattern of movement.

In other words, you can learn to play the piano

just as fast or maybe even faster

by just focusing on the sequence

that you are moving your digits,

your fingers and not the feedback.

Now, I’m sure there are music teachers out there

and piano teachers that are screaming,

no, you’re going to ruin the practice

that all of us have embedded in our minds

and in our students.

And I agree, at some point,

you need to start including feedback

about whether or not things sound correct.

But one of the beauties of skill learning

is that you can choose to parameterize it,

meaning you can choose to just focus on the motor sequence

or just focus on the sounds that are coming back

and then integrate those.

And so we hear a lot about chunking,

about breaking things down into their component parts.

But one of the biggest challenges for skill learning

is knowing where to place your attention.

So to dial out again,

we’re building a protocol across this episode,

early sessions, maybe it’s the first one,

maybe it’s the first 10, maybe it’s the first 100.

It depends on how many repetitions you’re packing in.

But during those initial sessions,

the key is to make many errors,

to let the reward process govern the plasticity,

let the errors open the plasticity.

And then after the learning sessions,

to let the brain go idle,

at least for a short period of time.

And of course, to maximize sleep.

As you start incorporating more sessions,

you start to gain some skill level.

Learning to harness and focus your attention

on particular features of the movement,

independent of the rewards and the feedback, right?

So the reward is no longer in the tone

coming from the piano,

or whether or not you struck the target correctly,

but simply the motor movement.

Focusing your, for instance, in a dart throw

on the action of your arm,

that is embedding the plasticity

in the motor pattern most deeply.

That’s what’s been shown by the scientific literature.

I’m sure there are coaches and teachers out there

that will entirely disagree with me.

And that’s great.

Please let me know what you prefer.

Let me know where you think this is wrong.

And it rarely happens,

but let me know where you think

this might be right as well.

So we’re breaking the learning process

down into its component parts.

As we get more and more skilled,

meaning as we make fewer and fewer errors

per a given session per unit time,

that’s when attention can start to migrate

from one feature, such as the motor sequence,

to another feature, which is perhaps one’s stance.

And another component of the sequence,

which would be the result that’s one getting

on a trial to trial basis, right?

So changing it up each time.

So maybe I serve the tennis ball

and I’m focusing on where the ball lands.

And then I’m focusing on the speed.

Then I’m focusing on my grip.

Then I’m focusing on my stance from trial to trial.

But until we’ve mastered the core motor movements,

which is done session to session,

that, at least according to the literature

that I have access to here, seems to be suboptimal.

So hopefully this is starting to make sense,

which is that these connections between upper motor neurons,

lower motor neurons, and central pattern generators,

you can’t attack them all at once.

You can’t try and change them all at once.

And so what we’re doing is we’re breaking things down

into their component parts.

Some of you may be wondering about speed of movement.

There are some data, meaning some decent papers out there,

showing that ultra slow movements,

performing a movement essentially in slow motion,

can be beneficial for enhancing the rate of skill learning.

However, at least from my read of the literature,

it appears that ultra slow movements should be performed

after some degree of proficiency has already been gained

in that particular movement.

Now, that’s not the way I would have thought about it.

I would have thought, well, you know,

if you’re learning how to do a proper kick

or a punch in martial arts

or something that ultra slow movements at first

are going to be the way that one can, you know,

best learn how to perform a movement.

And then you just gradually increase the speed.

And it turns out that’s not the case.

And I probably should have known that.

And you should probably know that

because it turns out that when you do ultra slow movements,

two things aren’t available to you.

One is the proprioceptive feedback is not accurate

because fast movements of limbs are very different

than slow movements of limbs.

So you don’t get the opportunity

to build in the proprioceptive feedback.

But the other reason why it doesn’t work

is that it’s too accurate.

You don’t generate errors.

And so the data that I was able to find

showed that very slow movements can be beneficial

if one is already proficient in a practice,

but very slow movements at the beginning

don’t allow you to learn more quickly

because you never generate errors.

And therefore the brain doesn’t, it’s not open for change.

The window for plasticity is never swung open, so to speak.

So brings us back to this theme

that errors allow for plasticity,

correct performance of movements

or semi-correct performance of movements

cues the synapses in the brain areas

and spinal circuits that need to change.

And then those changes occur in the period

immediately after skill learning and in sleep.

So super slow movements can be beneficial

once you already have some proficiency.

So this might be standing in your living room

and just in ultra slow motion,

performing your tennis serve,

learning to or thinking about how you’re adjusting

your elbow and your arm and the trajectory

exactly how you were taught by your tennis coach.

But trying to learn it that way from the outset

does not appear to be the best way to learn a skill.

When should you start to introduce slow learning?

Well, obviously talk to your coaches about this,

but if you’re doing this recreationally

or you don’t have a coach,

I realize many of you don’t.

I don’t have a coach for anything that I do.

I’m going to just navigating it

by using the scientific literature.

It appears that once you’re hitting success rates

of about 25 or 30%,

that’s where the super slow movements

can start to be beneficial.

But if you’re still performing things at a rate of,

you know, five or 10% correct and the rest are errors,

then the super slow movements

are probably not going to benefit you that much.

Also, super slow movements are not really applicable

to a lot of things.

For instance, you could imagine throwing a dart

super slow motion,

but if you actually try and throw an actual dart,

the dart’s just going to fall to the floor, obviously.

So there are a number of things like, you know,

baseball bat swing,

which you can practice in super slow motion.

But if you try and do that with an actual baseball

or softball or something like that,

that’s not going to give you any kind of feedback

about how effective it was.

So super slow movements or a decelerated movement

has its place,

but once you’re already performing things reasonably well,

like maybe 25 to 30% success rate.

You know, and I’ve tried this.

I actually, I struggle with basketball for whatever reason,

and my free throw is terrible.

So I’ve practiced free throws in super slow motion

and I nail them every time.

The problem is there’s no ball.

Some of you already have a fair degree of proficiency

of skill in a given practice or sport or instrument.

And if you’re in this sort of advanced intermediate

or advanced levels of proficiency for something,

there is a practice that you can find interesting data for

in the literature, which involves metronoming.

So this you’ll realize relates to generating repetitions

and it relates to the tone experiment

where it doesn’t really matter

what your attention is cued to,

as long as you are performing many, many reps

of the motor sequence.

You can use a metronome and obviously musicians do this,

but athletes can do this too.

You can use a metronome to set the cadence

of your repetitions.

Now for swimmers, there’s actually a device

I was able to find online.

I forget what the brand name was

and that’s not what this is about,

but that actually goes in the swim cap

that can cue you to when you need

to perform another stroke.

And for runners, there are other metronome type devices

that through headphones or through a tone in the room,

if you’re running indoors or on a treadmill,

will cue you to when you basically

you need to lift your heels.

And if you do that, what athletes find

is they can perform more repetitions,

they can generate more output, you can increase speed.

A number of really interesting things are being done

with auditory metronoming.

And then I’m involved in a little bit of work now

that hopefully I’ll be able to report back to you

about using stroboscopic metronoming.

So actually changing the speed of the visual environment.

These are fun experiments,

basically changing one’s perception

of how fast they’re moving through space

by playing with the visual system,

something for a future discussion.

But you can start to use auditory metronoming

for generating more movements per unit time

and generating more errors and therefore more successes

and more neuroplasticity.

There are a number of different apps out there.

I found several free apps

where you can set in a metronome pace.

So it might be tick, tick, tick, tick, tick, tick.

That’s a little fast for most things,

but you can imagine if this were darts

or this were golf swings,

that it might be tick, tick, tick, tick,

or something more like tick, tick.

And every time the metronome goes, you swing.

Every time the metronome goes, you throw a dart.

There are actually some wild experiments out there.

You know, there’s a world championship of cup stacking.

There’s a young lady who I saw could take

all these cups spread out on a table

and basically just stack them into the perfect pyramid

and the least amount of times all the kids go wild.

This is something I’d never thought to pursue

and frankly never will pursue

unless my life depends on it for some reason,

but it’s really impressive.

And if you look at the sequence,

because these have been recorded,

you can look this up on YouTube.

What you’ll find is that these expert cup stackers,

it’s just all about error elimination.

But there too, metronoming and auditory cues

can actually cue them to pick up the cups

faster than they would ordinarily and to learn to do that.

You can do this for anything.

I think cup stacking is probably not a skill

most of you are interested in doing,

but for any skill, if you figure out

at what rate you are performing repetitions per unit time

and you want to increase that slightly,

you set a metronome,

which is slightly faster than your current rate

and you just start generating more repetitions.

Now, what’s interesting about this and is cool

is it relates back to the experiment

from Lappe and colleagues,

which is your attention is now harnessed to the tone,

to the metronome,

not necessarily to what you’re doing

in terms of the motor movement.

And so really you need a bit of proficiency.

Again, this is for people who are intermediate

or advanced, intermediate or advanced.

But what you’re essentially doing

is you’re creating an outside pressure, a contingency,

so that you generate again, more errors.

So it’s all about the errors that you get.

Now, these aren’t errors where all the cups tumble

or you have to stop or you can’t keep up.

You have to set the pace just a little bit

beyond what you currently can do.

And when you do that,

you’re essentially forcing the nervous system

to make errors and correct the errors

inside of the session.

I find this really interesting

because what it means is again,

you’ve got sensory perception,

what you’re paying attention to,

proprioception, where your limbs are

and the motor neurons in your upper,

lower motor neurons and central pattern generators.

And you can’t pay attention to it.

Well, they’re my upper motor neurons.

They’re my lower motor neurons.

Forget that, you’re not going to do that.

You can’t pay attention to your proprioception too much.

That would be the super slow motion

would be the proprioception.

But you have to harness your attention to something.

And if you harness your attention

to this outside contingency,

this metronome that’s firing off and saying,

now, go, now, go, now, go.

Not only can you increase the number of repetitions,

errors and successes, but for some reason,

and we don’t know why,

but the regular cadence of the tone of the metronome

and the fact that you are anchoring your movements

to some external force, to some external pressure or cue

seems to accelerate the plasticity

and the changes and the acquisition of skills

beyond what it would be

if you just did the same number of repetitions

without that outside pressure.

We don’t know exactly what the mechanism is.

Presumably it’s neurochemical.

Like there’s something about keeping up with a timer

or with a pace that presumably, and I’m speculating here,

causes the release of particular chemicals.

But I think it’s really cool.

Metronomes, they’re totally inexpensive.

At least the ones that you use outside of water

are very inexpensive.

You can find these free apps.

You can use a musical metronome.

So metronomes are a powerful tool as well,

in particular for speed work.

So for sprinting or swimming or running

where the goal is to generate more strokes

or more efficient strokes or more steps, et cetera,

the rate of the metronome, obviously,

is going to be very important.

Sometimes you’re trying to lengthen your stride.

Sometimes you’re trying to take fewer strokes

but glide further in the pool, for instance.

But the value of occasionally

just increasing the number of repetitions,

the number of strokes or steps, et cetera, per unit time

is also that you’re training the central pattern generators

to operate at that higher speed.

One of the sports that’s kind of interesting to me

is speed walking.

It’s not one I engage in or ever plan to engage in.

But if you’ve ever tried to really speed walk,

it’s actually difficult to walk very, very fast

without breaking into a run.

All animals have these kind of crossover points

where you go, you know, I think of horses,

it’s like, what is it?

They trot, then they gallop.

I don’t know what’s the next thing.

I don’t know anything about horses

except that they’re beautiful and I like them very much.

But they break into a different kind of stride.

And that’s because you shift over

to different central pattern generators.

So when you’re walking or a horse is moving very slowly

and then it breaks into a jog and then into a full sprint

or I got to get gallop for the horse,

you’re actually engaging

different central pattern generators.

And those central pattern generators

always have a range of speeds

that they’re happiest to function at.

So with the metronoming for speed purposes,

what you do is you can basically bring the activity

of those central pattern generators

into their upper, upper range

and maybe even extend their range.

And there’s a fascinating biology

of how central pattern generators work together.

There’s coupling of central pattern generators, et cetera,

in order to achieve maximal speeds and et cetera.

It’s a topic for kind of an advanced session.

Costello loves this topic.

He just barked and he loves it so much he barked again.

In any event, the metronome is a powerful tool,

again, for more advanced practitioners

or for advanced intermediate practitioners.

But it’s interesting because it brings back the point

that what we put our attention to

while we’re skill learning is important

to the extent that it’s on one thing,

at least for the moment or trial to trial,

but that what we focus our attention on can be external,

it can be internal,

and ultimately the skill learning

is where all that is brought together.

So let’s talk about where skill learning occurs

in the nervous system.

And then I’m going to give you a really,

what I think is a really cool tool

that can increase flexibility and a range of motion

based on this particular brain area.

It’s a tool that I used.

And when I first heard about it,

I did not believe would work.

This is not a hack.

This is actually anchored deeply in the biology

of a particular brain region that we all have,

whose meaning is mini brain.

And that mini brain that we all have

is called your cerebellum.

The cerebellum is called the mini brain

because it’s in the back of your brain.

It looks like a little mini version

of the rest of your brain.

It’s an absolutely incredible structure

that’s involved in movement.

It also has a lot of non-movement associated functions.

In brief, the cerebellum gets input from your senses,

in particular your eyes,

and pays attention to where your eyes are in space,

what you’re looking at.

It basically takes information about three aspects

of your eyes and eye movements,

which are occurring when your head goes like this,

which is called pitch.

Okay, so this is pitch.

For those of you that are listening,

I’m just nodding up and down.

Then there’s yaw, which is like shaking your head no

from side to side.

And then there’s roll,

which is like sometimes if you see a primate,

like a marmoset or something,

they will roll their head when they look at you.

Actually, the reason they do that

is it helps generate depth perception.

It’s a kind of form of motion parallax

if you’re curious why they do that.

It’s not to look cute.

They do it because when they do that,

even if you’re stationary and they’re stationary,

they get better depth perception

as to how far away from them you are.

So you’ve got pitch, yaw, and roll.

And as you move your head and as you move your body

and you move through space,

the image on your retina moves,

pitch, yaw, and roll in some combination.

That information is relayed to your cerebellum.

So it’s rich with visual information.

There’s also a map of your body surface

and your movements and timing in the cerebellum.

So it’s an incredible structure

that brings together timing of movements,

which limbs are moving,

and has proprioceptive information.

It really is a mini brain.

It’s just the coolest little structure back there.

And in humans, it’s actually not that little.

It’s just an incredible structure.

Now, all this information is integrated there,

but what most people don’t tell us

is that a lot of learning of motor sequences,

of skill learning that involve timing

occurs in the cerebellum.

Now, you can’t really use that information

except to know that after you learn something pretty well,

it’s handed off or kind of handled by your cerebellum.

But there is something that you can do with your cerebellum

to increase range of motion and flexibility.

Much of our flexibility, believe it or not,

is not because our tendons are a particular length

or elasticity, although that plays some role.

It’s not because our muscles are short.

I don’t know what that would even mean.

Some people have longer muscle bellies

or shorter muscle bellies,

but your muscles always essentially span the entire length

of the bone or limb or close to it,

along with your tendons.

But it has to do with the neural innervation of muscle

and the fact that when muscles are elongated,

there’s a point at which they won’t stretch out any longer

and the nerves fire and they shut down.

You actually have inhibitory pathways

that prevent you from contracting the muscles

or from extending them, from stretching them out anymore.

So you can do this right now.

If you’re driving, don’t do it

because unless you have a self-driving car,

you’ll need to take your hands off the steering wheel.

But because of the way that vision

and your muscles are represented in your cerebellum,

it turns out that your range of visual motion

and your range of vision,

literally how wide a field of view you take,

impacts how far you can extend your limbs.

Okay, so we’ll talk about this in a second,

about exactly how to do this and explore this.

But as you move through space, as you walk forward,

or you walk backward, or you tilt your head,

or you learn a skill,

or you just operate in the normal ways throughout your day,

driving, biking, et cetera,

your eyes are generating spontaneous movements

to offset visual slip.

In other words, you don’t see the world as blurry

even though you’re moving

because your eyes are generating

low compensatory eye movements to offset your motion.

So if I spin, we could do this experiment.

There’s a fun experiment we do with medical students

where you spin them around in a chair

with their eyes closed,

and then you stop and you have them open their eyes

and their eyes are going like this,

doo-doo-doo-doo-doo-doo-doo, a nice stagmas.

I don’t suggest you do this experiment.

When we were kids, we did a different experiment,

which was to take a stick

and to look at the top of the stick

and to spin around on the lawn,

looking at the top of the stick,

then put it down on the ground and try and jump over it.

And you end up like jumping to the side,

you miss the thing entirely.

The reason those two quote unquote experiments,

which I hope you don’t do or force somebody else to do,

the reason they work is because normally your eye movements

and your balance and your limb movements are coordinated.

But when you spin around looking up at the stick,

what you’re doing is you’re fixating your eyes

on one location while you’re moving.

And then when you stop,

those two mechanisms are completely uncoupled

and it’s like being thrown into outer space.

Never been to outer space,

but probably something like that,

low gravity, zero gravity.

If you spin around in your chair with your eyes closed,

you’re not giving the visual input that you’re spinning.

And then you open the eyes

and then the eyes only have the,

what we call the vestibular signal,

your eyes jolting back and forth, back and forth.

Again, these aren’t experiments you need to do

because I just told you the result.

However, if you want to extend your range of motion,

you can do that by, these things always look goofy,

but at this point,

I’m just kind of used to doing these things.

If I want to extend my range of movement,

first, I want to measure my range of motion.

So I’m trying to, if you’re listening,

what I’m doing is I’m stretching out my arms

from like a T on either side,

and I’m trying to push them as far back as I can,

which for me is, feels like it’s in line with my shoulders

and I can’t get much further.

I’m not really super flexible,

nor am I particularly inflexible, at least physically.

So what I would then do is stop.

I would move my eyes to the far periphery, right?

So I’m moving my eyes all the way to the left

while keeping my head and body stationary.

I’m trying to look over my left shoulder as far as I can,

then off to the right.

It’s a little awkward to do this.

Then up, then down,

but I’m mostly going to just focus on left and then right.

Now, what that’s doing is it’s sending a signal

to my cerebellum that my field of view

is way over to there and way over to there.

Remember, your visual attention has an aperture.

It can be narrow or it can be broad.

And I’ve talked about some of the benefits

of taking a broad visual aperture

in order to relax the nervous system.

This is just moving my eyes, not my head,

like I just did for a second, from side to side.

Now I can retest, and actually you get about

a five to 15 degree increase in your range of motion.

Now I’m doing this for you.

You can say, well, you know, he gamed it

because he knew the result that he was hoping for,

but you can try this, okay?

So, and you can do this for legs too, right?

You can do this for any limb essentially.

And that’s, it’s purely cerebellar.

And it’s because the proprioceptive visual

and limb movement feedback converge in their,

in the ways that we control our muscle spindles

and the way we control the muscle fibers and the tendons.

And essentially you can get bigger range of motion.

So I actually will warm up before exercise

or for before skill learning

by both doing movements for my body,

but also moving my eyes from side to side

in order to generate larger range of motion.

If range of motion is something that I’m interested in.

So that’s a fun one that you can play with a little bit.

And it’s purely cerebellar.

Some other time we’ll get back into cerebellar function.

There’s all sorts of just incredible stuff

that you can do with cerebellum.

I talked in an earlier episode on neuroplasticity

about how you can disrupt your vestibular world.

In other words, by getting into modes of acceleration,

moving through space where you’re tilted in certain ways,

it can open up the windows for plasticity

and yet other ways.

So you can check that out.

It’s one of the earlier episodes on neuroplasticity.

Everything’s timestamped.

But meanwhile, if you want to expand your range of motion

before doing skill learning or afterward,

this is a fun one.

It’s also kind of neat

because I have this kind of aversion to stretching work.

It never seems like something I want to do.

And so I always put it off.

So if I start with the visual practice

of expanding my field of view

off to one side or the other side or up or down,

then what I find is I’m naturally more flexible.

I’m not naturally more flexible.

What’s happened is I’ve expanded my range of motion.

Let’s talk about visualization and mental rehearsal.

I’ve been asked about this a lot.

And I think it relates back to that kind of a matrix

Hollywood idea that we can just be embedded with a skill.

Although in this case, in fairness,

visualization involves some work.

And I’ve talked about this on an earlier episode

that some people find it very hard

to mentally visualize things.

And some people find it very easy.

There was great work that was done in the 1960s

by Roger Shepard at Stanford and by others,

looking at people’s ability

to rotate three-dimensional objects in their mind.

And some people are really good at this

and some people are less good at this.

And one can get better at it by repeating it.

But the question we’re going to deal with today is,

does it help?

Does it let you learn things faster?

And indeed the answer appears to be yes, it can.

However, despite what you’ve heard, it is not as good.

It is not a total replacement

for physical performance itself.

Okay, so I’m going to be really concrete about this.

I hear all the time that just imagining contracting a muscle

can lead to the same gains

as actually contracting that muscle.

Just imagining a skill can lead to the same increases

in performance as actually executing that skill.

And that’s simply not the case.

However, it can supplement or support physical training

and skill learning in ways that are quite powerful.

One of the more interesting studies on this

was from Ranganathan et al.

Forgive me for the pronunciation.

This was a slightly older paper, 2004,

but nonetheless was one that I thought

had particularly impressive results

and included all the appropriate controls, et cetera.

And what they did is they looked at 30 subjects.

They divide them into different groups.

They had one group perform essentially finger flexion.

So it was actually just sort of the,

imagine if you’re just listening to this,

the, you know, come here, finger movement.

They also had elbow flexion.

So it’s a bicep curl type movement.

And they either had subjects

do a actual physical movement against resistance

or to imagine moving their finger

or their wrist towards the shoulder,

meaning at the bending at the elbow

towards actual resistance.

Just to make a long story short,

what they found was that there were increases

in this finger adduction strength,

abduction, excuse me, strength of about 35%

and the elbow flexion strength by about 13.5%,

which are pretty impressive

considering that it was just done mentally.

So they had people imagine moving against a weight,

a very heavy weight,

or had imagined people moving their wrist

towards their shoulder against a very heavy weight.

But again, they weren’t doing it.

They were just imagining it.

Other experiments looked at the brain

and what was happening in the brain during this time.

So we’ll talk about that in a moment.

But essentially what they found were

improvements in strength of anywhere from 13.5 to 35%.

However, the actual physical training group,

the groups that actually moved their wrist

or move their finger against an actual physical weight

had improvements of about 53%.

So this repeats over and over throughout the literature.

Mental rehearsal can cause increases in strength.

It can create increases in skill acquisition and learning,

but they are never as great

if done alone as compared

to the actual physical execution of those movements

or the physical movement of those weights,

which shouldn’t come as so surprising.

However, if we step back and we say,

well, what is the source of this improvement?

You might not care what the source is

because I could tell you it’s one brain area

or another brain area, what difference would it make?

But again, if you can understand mechanism a little bit,

you’re in a position to create newer

and even better protocols.

What mental rehearsal appears to do

is engage the activity of those upper motor neurons

that we talked about way back

at the beginning of the episode.

Remember, you have upper motor neurons

that control deliberate action.

You’ve got lower motor neurons

that actually connect to the muscles

and move those muscles

and you have central pattern generators.

Mental rehearsal, closing one’s eyes typically

and thinking about a particular sequence of movement

and visualizing it in one’s quote unquote mind’s eye

creates activation of the upper motor neurons

that’s very similar if not the same as the actual movement.

And that makes sense because the upper motor neurons

are all about the command for movement.

They are not the ones that actually execute the movement.

Okay?

Remember, upper motor neurons are the ones

that generate the command for movement,

not the actual movement.

The ones that generate the actual movement

are the lower motor neurons

and the central pattern generators.

So visualization is a powerful tool.

How can you use visualization?

Well, in this study,

they had people perform this five days a week.

I believe that it was 15, yes,

it was 15 minutes per day, five days a week for 12 weeks.

So that’s a lot of mental rehearsal.

It’s not a ton of time each day, 15 minutes per day,

but sitting down, closing your eyes

and imagining going through a particular skill practice

or moving a weight.

Maybe it’s playing keys on a piano,

if that’s your thing, or strings on a guitar.

For 15 minutes a day, five days per week for 12 weeks

is considerable.

I think most people,

given the fact that the actual practice,

the physical practice is going to lead

to larger improvements, greater improvements

than would the mental training

would opt for the actual physical training.

But of course, if you’re on a plane

and you don’t have access to your guitar

and you’re certainly not going to be sprinting

up and down the aisle,

or you are very serious about your craft

and you want to accelerate performance of your craft

or strength increases or something of that sort,

then augmenting or adding in the visualization training

very likely will compound the effects

of the actual physical training.

There are not a lot of studies looking at how visualization

on top of pure physical training

can increase the rates of learning

and consolidation of learning, et cetera.

It’s actually a hard study to do

because it’s hard to control for,

because what would you do in its place?

You would probably add actual physical training

and then that’s always going to lead to greater effects.

So the point is,

if you want to use visualization training, great,

but forget the idea that visualization training

is as good as the actual behavior.

You hear this all the time.

People say, do you know that if you imagine an experience

to your brain and to your body,

it’s exactly the same as the actual experience.

Absolutely not.

This is not the way the nervous system works.

I’m sorry, I don’t mean to burst anybody’s bubble,

but your bubble is made of myths.

And the fact of the matter is that the brain,

when it executes movement,

is generating proprioceptive feedback.

And that proprioceptive feedback is critically involved

in generating our sense of the experience

and in things like learning.

So I don’t say this because I don’t like the idea

that visualization couldn’t work.

In fact, visualization does work,

but it doesn’t work as well.

It doesn’t create the same milieu,

the same chemical milieu, the same environment

as actual physically engaging in the behavior,

the skill, the resistance training, et cetera.

And I’d be willing to wager that the same is true

for experiences of all kinds.

PTSD is this incredibly unfortunate circumstance

in which there’s a replay often of the traumatic event

that feels very real.

But that’s not to say that the replay itself

is the same as the actual event.

And of course, PTSD needs to be dealt with

with the utmost level of seriousness.

It should be treated.

In fact, my lab works on these sorts of things.

But my point about visualization and imagining something

not being the same as the actual experience

is grounded in this idea of proprioception

and the fact that feedback to the cerebellum,

the cerebellum talking to other areas of the brain

are critically involved in communicating

to the rest of our nervous system

that not just that we believe something is happening,

but something is actually happening.

And in the case of muscle loads,

muscles actually feeling tension,

the actual feeling of tension in the muscle,

the contracting of the muscle under that tension

is part of the important adaptation process.

In a future episode, we’ll talk about hypertrophy

and how that works at the level of upper motor neurons,

lower motor neurons, and muscle itself.

But for now, just know that visualization can work.

It doesn’t work as well as real physical training

and practice, but these effects of 35% or 13.5% increases

are pretty considerable.

They’re just not as great as the 53% increases

that come from actual physical training.

For those of you that are interested

in some of the skill learning

that more relates to musical training,

but also how cadence and metronoming and tones, et cetera,

can support physical learning.

If you’re interested in that, you aficionados,

there is a wonderful review

also published in the journal, Neuron.

Again, excellent journal by Herholtz and Zatore.

That’s H-E-R-H-O-L-Z and Zatore, Z-A-T-O-R-E-T-O-R-E.

That really describes in detail how musical training

can impact all sorts of different things

and how cadence training, whether or not with tones

or auditory feedback and things of that sort,

carries over to not just instrumental music training,

but also physical skill learning of various kinds.

So if you want to do the deep dive,

that would be the place.

You can find it easily online.

It’s available as a complete article free of charge,

et cetera.

Many of you are probably asking what can I take

in order to accelerate skill learning?

Well, the conditions are going to vary,

but motivation is key.

You have to show up to the training session

motivated enough to focus your attention

and to perform a lot of repetitions

in the training sequence.

That’s just a prerequisite, all right?

There’s no pill that’s going to allow you

to do fewer repetitions

and extract more learning out of fewer repetitions.

It’s actually more a question of what are the conditions

that you can create for yourself

such that you can generate more repetitions per unit time?

I think that’s the right way to think about it.

What are the conditions that you can create for yourself

in your mind and in your body

that are going to allow you to focus?

And I’ve talked about focus and plasticity and motivation

in previous episodes.

Please see those episodes if you have questions about that.

I detail a lot of tools and the underlying science.

So for some people, it might be drinking a cup or two

of coffee and getting hydrated before the training session.

For some of you, it might be avoiding coffee

because it makes you too jittery

and your attention jumps all over the place.

It’s going to vary tremendously.

There’s no real, there is no magic pill

that’s going to allow you to get more out of less.

That’s just not going to happen.

It’s simply not going to happen.

You’re not going to get more learning

out of fewer repetitions or less time.

However, there are a few compounds

that I think are worth mentioning

because of their ability to improve

the actual physical performance,

the actual execution of certain types of movements.

And some of these have also been shown

to improve cognitive function,

especially in older populations.

So I’d be remiss if I didn’t at least mention them.

I’m only going to mention one today.

In fact, the one that’s particularly interesting

and for which there really are a lot of data is alpha-GPC.

And I’m going to attempt to pronounce

what alpha-GPC actually is.

It’s alpha-glycerophospho-choline, right?

Alpha-GPC, alpha-glycerophospho-choline.

See, if I keep doing it over and over repetitions,

alpha-glycerophospho-choline.

There, I made an error.

Okay, so the point is that alpha-GPC,

which is at least in the United States

is sold over the counter,

typically is taken in dosages of about 300 to 600 milligrams.

That’s a single dose or have been shown

to do a number of things that for some of you

might be beneficial.

One is to enhance power output.

So if you’re engaging in something like shot put throwing

or resistance training or sprinting

or something where you have to generate a lot of power,

maybe you’re doing rock climbing,

but you’re working on a particular aspect

of your rock climbing that involves

generating a lot of force, a lot of power.

Well then, in theory, alpha-GPC could be beneficial to you.

For the cognitive effects, the dosages are much higher,

up to 1200 milligrams daily divided into three doses

of 400 milligrams is what the studies

that I was able to find show or used.

The effects on cognitive decline are described as notable.

Notable meaning several studies showed a significant

but modest effect in offsetting cognitive decline,

in particular in older populations and some populations

even with some reported neurodegeneration.

Power output was notable.

How notable?

What does that mean, notable?

A study noted a 14% increase in power output.

That’s pretty substantial, 14% if you think about it,

but it wasn’t like a doubling or something of that sort.

Believe it or not, the symptoms of Alzheimer’s

have been shown at least among the nutraceuticals

of which alpha-GPC is to significantly improve cognition

in people with Alzheimer’s.

Now, this episode isn’t about cognitive decline

and longevity, we will talk about that,

but this is a so-called another effect of alpha-GPC.

Fat oxidation is increased by alpha-GPC,

growth hormone release is promoted by alpha-GPC,

although to a small degree.

So as you can see, things like alpha-GPC in particular

when they are combined with low levels of caffeine

can have these effects of improving power output,

can improve growth hormone release,

can improve fat oxidation.

All these things in theory can support skill learning,

but what they’re really doing

is they’re adjusting the foundation

upon which you are going to execute

these many, many repetitions, okay?

The same thing would be said for caffeine itself.

If that’s something that motivates you

and gets you out of a chair

to actually do the physical training,

then that’s something that can perhaps improve

or enhance the rate of skill learning

and how well you retain those skills.

Now, on a previous episode, I talked about,

and this was the episode on epinephrine, on adrenaline,

I talked about how for mental, for cognitive learning,

it makes sense to spike epinephrine,

to bump epinephrine levels up,

adrenaline levels up after cognitive learning.

For physical learning, it appears to be the opposite,

that if you are, if caffeine is in your practice

or if you decide to try alpha-GPC,

that you would want to do that before the training,

take it before the training, use it,

its effects should extend into the training,

presumably throughout,

and then afterward, if you’re thinking about following

some of the protocols that we discussed today,

that you would use some sort of idle time

where the brain can replay these motor sequences in reverse,

and then of course, you want to do things

to optimize your sleep.

A lot of the questions I get are about

how different protocols and things that I describe

start to collide with one another.

So let’s say, for instance, you go to bed at 10.30

and you’re going to do your skill training at 9.30,

well, taking a lot of caffeine then

is not going to be a good idea

because it’s going to compromise your sleep.

So I’m not here to design the perfect schedule for you

because everyone’s situations vary.

So the things to optimize are repetitions, failures,

more repetitions, more failures,

at the offset of training, having some idle time

that could be straight into sleep,

or it could be simply letting the brain just go idle

for five to 10 minutes,

me not focusing on anything, not scrolling social media,

not emailing, ideally not even talking to somebody,

just lying down or sitting quietly with your eyes closed,

letting those motor sequences replay.

Then we talked about how one can come back

for additional training sessions,

use things like metronoming,

where you’re cueing your attention to some external cue,

some stimulus, in this case,

an auditory stimulus most likely,

and trying to generate more repetitions per unit time.

So again, it’s repetitions and errors, that’s key.

And then we also talked about some things that you can do

involving cerebellar neurophysiology

to extend range of motion,

if that’s what’s limiting for you,

or to use visualization to augment the practice,

or let’s say your particular skill involves nice weather

and it’s raining or snowing outside

and you can’t get outside, a thunderstorm,

then that’s where visualization training

might be a good replacement under those conditions,

or in most cases is going to be the kind of thing

that you’re going to want to do

in addition to the actual physical skill

or strength training session done,

at least in the study that we described,

for 15 minutes a day, five days a week,

over a period of 10 to 12 weeks or so.

So hopefully that makes it clear.

Today, we’ve covered a lot of mechanism.

We talked so much about the different motor pathways,

central pattern generators.

So you now are armed with a lot of information

about how you generate movement.

And I like to think that you’re also armed

with a lot of information about how to design protocols

that are optimized for you,

or if you’re a coach for your trainees

in order to optimize their learning

of skills of various kinds.

Today, we focused almost entirely on motor skills,

things like musical skills or physical skills.

These have some overlap.

They’re partially overlapping with neuroplasticity

for learning things like languages, or math,

or engineering, or neuroscience for that matter.

Before we depart,

I just want to make sure that I return to a concept,

which is the ultradian cycle.

Ultradian cycles are these 90-minute cycles

that we go through throughout sleep and wakefulness

that are optimal for learning and attention

in the waking state.

They are the stages of sleep

in which we have either predominantly slow-wave sleep

or REM sleep.

Some of you who have been following this podcast

for a while might be asking,

well, should a physical practice be 90 minutes?

That’s going to depend because with physical practices,

oftentimes, for instance, with strength training,

that might be too long.

You’re not going to be able to generate enough force output

for it to be worthwhile.

For golfing, I don’t know.

I’ve never played golf,

although my friends that play golf,

they disappear onto the golf course for many hours.

So I know there’s a lot of walking and driving

and other stuff.

I even hear that somebody carries your stuff around for you.

Sometimes, not always, but it’s going to differ.

A four-hour golf game,

you’re probably not swinging the golf club for four hours.

So it’s going to depend.

I would say that the ultradian cycle

is not necessarily a good constraint

for skill learning in most cases.

And I should say that for those of you

that are short on time or have limited amounts of time,

10 minutes of maximum repetitions,

maximum focus skill learning work

is going to be very beneficial.

Whereas two hours of kind of haphazard,

not really focused work

or where you’re not generating very many repetitions

because you’re doing a few repetitions,

then you’re texting on your phone

or paying attention to something else,

that’s not going to be beneficial.

It’s really about the density of training

inside of a session.

So I think you should let the,

work toward maximal or near maximal density of repetitions

and failures, provided they’re failures

you can perform safely

in order to accelerate skill learning.

And don’t let some arbitrary,

or in this case, the ultradian constraint

prevent you from engaging in that practice.

In other words, get the work in,

get as much work done as you can per unit time.

And based on the science,

based on things that I’ve seen,

based on things that I’m now involved in

with various communities,

you will see the skill improve vastly at various stages.

Sometimes it’s a little bit stutter start.

It’s not always a linear improvement,

but you will see incredible improvement in skill.

If you’re enjoying this podcast

and you’re finding the information interesting

and or of use to you,

please subscribe on YouTube.

That really helps us.

As well, please subscribe and download the episodes

on Apple and Spotify.

On Apple, you also have the opportunity

to leave us up to a five-star review,

if you think we deserve that.

And on YouTube,

please hit the thumbs up button if you liked the episode

and please give us feedback.

Place your feedback in the comment section.

That’s a place to tell us how we’re doing,

but also to ask us questions.

We read all the comments.

It takes us some time to work through them,

but we read all of them

and we use your comments and your feedback

to sculpt the content and the direction of future episodes.

Another great way to support us

is to check out our sponsors

that we mentioned at the beginning of the podcast.

And as mentioned at the beginning of today’s episode,

we are now partnered with Momentous Supplements

because they make single ingredient formulations

that are of the absolute highest quality

and they ship international.

If you go to livemomentous.com slash Huberman,

you will find many of the supplements

that have been discussed on various episodes

of the Huberman Lab podcast

and you will find various protocols

related to those supplements.

And last but not least,

I want to thank you for your time and attention.

I very much appreciate your interest in neuroscience

and in physiology and in tools

that are informed by neuroscience and physiology.

Today, we talked all about skill learning.

I hope that you’ll consider the information.

You might even decide to try some of these tools.

If you do, please let us know your results with them.

Give us feedback in the comments.

And as always, thank you for your interest in science.

Thank you.

comments powered by Disqus