Huberman Lab - Dr. Jack Feldman: Breathing for Mental & Physical Health & Performance

Welcome to the Huberman Lab Podcast,

where we discuss science

and science-based tools for everyday life.

I’m Andrew Huberman,

and I’m a professor of neurobiology and ophthalmology

at Stanford School of Medicine.

Today, my guest is Dr. Jack Feldman.

Dr. Jack Feldman is a distinguished professor

of neurobiology at the University of California, Los Angeles.

He is known for his pioneering work

on the neuroscience of breathing.

We are all familiar with breathing

and how essential breathing is to life.

We require oxygen,

and it is only by breathing that we can bring oxygen

to all the cells of our brain and body.

However, as the work from Dr. Feldman and colleagues

tells us, breathing is also fundamental

to organ health and function

at an enormous number of other levels.

In fact, how we breathe, including how often we breathe,

the depth of our breathing,

and the ratio of inhales to exhales

actually predicts how focused we are,

how easily we get into sleep,

how easily we can exit from sleep.

Dr. Feldman gets credit for the discovery

of the two major brain centers

that control the different patterns of breathing.

Today, you’ll learn about those brain centers

and the patterns of breathing they control,

and how those different patterns of breathing

influence all aspects of your mental and physical life.

What’s especially wonderful about Dr. Feldman and his work

is that it not only points to the critical role

of respiration in disease, in health, and in daily life,

but he’s also a practitioner.

He understands how to leverage particular aspects

of the breathing process in order to bias the brain

to be in particular states that can benefit us all.

Whether or not you are a person

who already practices breath work,

or whether or not you’re somebody

who simply breathes to stay alive,

by the end of today’s discussion,

you’re going to understand a tremendous amount

about how the breathing system works

and how you can leverage that breathing system

toward particular goals in your life.

Dr. Feldman shares with us

his own particular breathing protocols that he uses,

and he suggests different avenues for exploring respiration

in ways that can allow you, for instance,

to be more focused for work,

to disengage from work and high stress endeavors,

to calm down quickly.

And indeed, he explains not only how to do that,

but all the underlying science

in ways that will allow you to customize

your own protocols for your needs.

All the guests that we bring on the Huberman Lab Podcast

are considered at the very top of their fields.

Today’s guest, Dr. Feldman,

is not only at the top of his field, he founded the field.

Prior to his coming into neuroscience

from the field of physics,

there really wasn’t much information

about how the brain controls breathing.

There was a little bit of information,

but we can really credit Dr. Feldman and his laboratory

for identifying the particular brain areas

that control different patterns of breathing

and how that information can be leveraged

towards health, high performance,

and for combating disease.

So today’s conversation,

you’re going to learn a tremendous amount

from the top researcher in this field.

It’s a really wonderful and special opportunity

to be able to share his knowledge with you.

And I know that you’re not only going to enjoy it,

but you are going to learn a tremendous amount.

Before we begin, I’d like to emphasize

that this podcast is separate

from my teaching and research roles at Stanford.

It is however, part of my desire and effort

to bring zero cost to consumer information about science

and science related tools to the general public.

In keeping with that theme,

I’d like to thank the sponsors of today’s podcast.

Our first sponsor is Athletic Greens.

Athletic Greens is an all-in-one

vitamin mineral probiotic drink.

I’ve been taking Athletic Greens since 2012.

So I’m delighted that they’re sponsoring the podcast.

The reason I started taking Athletic Greens

and the reason I still take Athletic Greens

once or twice a day is that it helps me cover

all of my basic nutritional needs.

It makes up for any deficiencies that I might have.

In addition, it has probiotics,

which are vital for microbiome health.

I’ve done a couple of episodes now

on the so-called gut microbiome

and the ways in which the microbiome interacts

with your immune system, with your brain to regulate mood,

and essentially with every biological system

relevant to health throughout your brain and body.

With Athletic Greens, I get the vitamins I need,

the minerals I need, and the probiotics

to support my microbiome.

If you’d like to try Athletic Greens,

you can go to athleticgreens.com slash Huberman

and claim a special offer.

They’ll give you five free travel packs

plus a year supply of vitamin D3K2.

There are a ton of data now showing that vitamin D3

is essential for various aspects of our brain

and body health, even if we’re getting a lot of sunshine.

Many of us are still deficient in vitamin D3

and K2 is also important because it regulates things

like cardiovascular function, calcium in the body,

and so on.

Again, go to athleticgreens.com slash Huberman

to claim the special offer of the five free travel packs

and the year supply of vitamin D3K2.

Today’s episode is also brought to us by Element.

Element is an electrolyte drink that has everything you need

and nothing you don’t.

That means the exact ratios of electrolytes are in Element,

and those are sodium, magnesium, and potassium,

but it has no sugar.

I’ve talked many times before on this podcast

about the key role of hydration and electrolytes

for nerve cell function, neuron function,

as well as the function of all the cells

and all the tissues and organ systems of the body.

If we have sodium, magnesium, and potassium

present in the proper ratios,

all of those cells function properly

and all our bodily systems can be optimized.

If the electrolytes are not present and if hydration is low,

we simply can’t think as well as we would otherwise,

our mood is off, hormone systems go off,

our ability to get into physical action,

to engage in endurance and strength

and all sorts of other things is diminished.

So with Element, you can make sure

that you’re staying on top of your hydration

and that you’re getting the proper ratios of electrolytes.

If you’d like to try Element, you can go to drinkelement,

that’s lmnt.com slash Huberman,

and you’ll get a free Element sample pack

with your purchase, they’re all delicious.

So again, if you want to try Element,

you can go to elementlmnt.com slash Huberman.

Today’s episode is also brought to us by Thesis.

Thesis makes what are called nootropics,

which means smart drugs.

Now, to be honest, I am not a fan of the term nootropics,

I don’t believe in smart drugs in the sense that

I don’t believe that there’s any one substance

or collection of substances that can make us smarter.

I do believe based on science, however,

that there are particular neural circuits

and brain functions that allow us to be more focused,

more alert, access creativity, be more motivated, et cetera.

That’s just the way that the brain works,

different neural circuits for different brain states.

Thesis understands this.

And as far as I know, they’re the first nootropics company

to create targeted nootropics for specific outcomes.

I’ve been using Thesis for more than six months now,

and I can confidently say that their nootropics

have been a total game changer.

My go-to formula is the clarity formula,

or sometimes I’ll use their energy formula before training.

To get your own personalized nootropic starter kit,

go online to takethesis.com slash Huberman,

take a three-minute quiz,

and Thesis will send you four different formulas

to try in your first month.

That’s takethesis.com slash Huberman,

and use the code Huberman at checkout

for 10% off your first order.

I’m pleased to announce that the Huberman Lab Podcast

is now partnered with Momentus Supplements.

We partnered with Momentus for several important reasons.

First of all, they ship internationally

because we know that many of you are located

outside of the United States.

Second of all, and perhaps most important,

the quality of their supplements is second to none,

both in terms of purity and precision

of the amounts of the ingredients.

Third, we’ve really emphasized supplements

that are single ingredient supplements,

and that are supplied in dosages

that allow you to build a supplementation protocol

that’s optimized for cost,

that’s optimized for effectiveness,

and that you can add things and remove things

from your protocol in a way

that’s really systematic and scientific.

If you’d like to see the supplements

that we partner with Momentus on,

you can go to livemomentus.com slash Huberman.

There you’ll see those supplements,

and just keep in mind that we are constantly expanding

the library of supplements available through Momentus

on a regular basis.

Again, that’s livemomentus.com slash Huberman.

One quick mention before we dive into the conversation

with Dr. Feldman.

During today’s episode,

we discuss a lot of breathwork practices,

and by the end of the episode,

all of those will be accessible to you.

However, I’m aware that there are a number of people

out there that want to go even further

into the science and practical tools of breathwork,

and for that reason, I want to mention a resource to you.

There is a cost associated with this resource,

but it’s a terrific platform

for learning about breathwork practices

and for building a number of different routines

that you can do or that you could teach.

It’s called Our Breathwork Collective.

I’m not associated with the Breathwork Collective,

but Dr. Feldman is an advisor to the group,

and they offer daily live guided breathing sessions

and an on-demand library

that you can practice anytime, free workshops on breathwork,

and these are really developed by experts in the field,

including Dr. Feldman.

So as I mentioned, I’m not on their advisory board,

but I do know them and their work,

and it is of the utmost quality.

So anyone wanting to learn or teach breathwork

could really benefit from this course, I believe.

If you’d like to learn more,

you can click on the link in the show notes

or visit ourbreathcollective.com slash Huberman

and use the code Huberman at checkout,

and if you do that,

they’ll offer you $10 off the first month.

Again, it’s ourbreathcollective.com slash Huberman

to access the Our Breath Collective.

And now for my conversation with Dr. Jack Feldman.

Thanks for joining me today.

It’s a pleasure to be here, Andrew.

Yeah, it’s been a long time coming.

You’re my go-to source for all things respiration.

I mean, I breathe on my own,

but when I want to understand how I breathe

and how the brain and breathing interact,

you’re the person I call.

Well, I’ll do my best.

As you know, there’s a lot that we don’t understand,

which still keeps me employed and engaged,

but we do know a lot.

Why don’t we start off by just talking about

what’s involved in generating breath?

And if you would,

could you comment on some of the mechanisms

for rhythmic breathing versus non-rhythmic breathing?

Okay, so on the mechanical side,

which is obvious to everyone,

we want to have air flow in, inhale,

and we need to have air flow out.

And the reason we need to do this

is because for body metabolism, we need oxygen.

And when oxygen is utilized

through the aerobic metabolic process,

we produce carbon dioxide.

And so we have to get rid of the carbon dioxide

that we produce,

in particular because the carbon dioxide

affects the acid-base balance of the blood, the pH.

And all living cells are very sensitive

to what the pH value is.

So your body is very interested in regulating that pH.

So we have to have enough oxygen for our normal metabolism,

and we have to get rid of the CO2 that we produce.

So how do we generate this air flow?

Well, the air comes into the lungs.

We have to expand the lungs.

And as the lungs expand,

basically it’s like a balloon that you would pull apart.

The pressure inside that balloon drops,

and air will flow into the balloon.

So we put pressure on the lung to pull it apart.

That lowers the pressure in the air sacs called alveoli,

and air will flow in because pressure outside the body

is higher than pressure inside the body

when you’re doing this expansion, when you’re inhaling.

What produces that?

Well, the principal muscle is the diaphragm,

which is sitting inside the body just below the lung.

And when you want to inhale,

you basically contract the diaphragm and it pulls it down.

And as it pulls it down,

it’s inserting pressure forces on the lung.

The lung wants to expand.

At the same time, the ribcage is gonna rotate up and out,

and therefore expanding the cavity, the thoracic cavity.

At the end of inspiration,

under normal conditions when you’re at rest,

you just relax, and it’s like pulling on a spring.

You pull down a spring and you let go and it relaxes.

So you inhale and you exhale.

Inhale, relax, and exhale.

So the exhale is passive?

At rest, it’s passive.

We’ll get into what happens when you need to increase

the amount of air you’re bringing in

because your ventilation,

your metabolism goes up like doing exercise.

Now, the muscles themselves,

the skeletal muscles don’t do anything

unless the nervous system tells them to do something.

And when the nervous system tells them to do something,

they contract.

So there are specialized neurons in the spinal cord

and above the spinal cord,

the region called the brainstem,

which go to respiratory muscles,

in particular for inspiration,

the diaphragm and the external intercostal muscles

in the ribcage, and they contract.

So these respiratory muscles,

these inspiratory muscles become active.

And they become active for a period of time.

Then they become silent.

And when they become silent,

the muscles then relax back

to their original resting level.

Where does that activity in these neurons

that innovate the muscle, which are called motor neurons,

where does that originate?

Well, this was a question that’s been bandied around

for thousands of years.

And when I was a beginning assistant professor,

it was fairly high priority for me

to try and figure that out

because I wanted to understand

where this rhythm of breathing was coming from.

And you couldn’t know where it was coming from

until you knew where it was coming from.

And I didn’t phrase that properly.

You couldn’t understand how it was being done

until you know where to look.

So we did a lot of experiments,

which I can go into detail,

and finally found there was a region in the brainstem.

That’s, once again, this region

sort of above the spinal cord,

which was critical for generating this rhythm.

It’s called the pre-Butzinger complex.

And we could talk about how that was named.

This small site, which contains in humans

a few thousand neurons,

it’s located on either side,

and it works in tandem.

And every breath begins

with neurons in this region beginning to be active.

And those neurons then connect,

ultimately, to these motor neurons

going to the diaphragm and to the external intercostals,

causing them to be active

and causing this inspiratory effort.

When the neurons in the pre-Butzinger complex

finish their burst of activity,

then inspiration stops,

and then you begin to exhale

because of this passive recall of the lung and rib cage.

Could I just briefly interrupt you

to ask a few quick questions

before we move forward in this very informative answer?

And the two questions are,

is there anything known about the activation

of the diaphragm and the intercostal muscles

between the ribs as it relates to nose

versus mouth breathing?

Or are they activated in the equivalent way

regardless of whether or not someone is breathing

through their nose or mouth?

I don’t think we fully have the answer to that.

Clearly, there are differences

between nasal and mouth breathing.

At rest, the tendency is to do nasal breathing

because the air flows that are necessary

for normal breathing is easily managed

by passing through the nasal cavities.

However, when your ventilation needs to increase,

like during exercise, you need to move more air,

you do that through your mouth

because the airways are much larger then,

and therefore you can move much more air.

But at the level of the intercostals and the diaphragm,

their contraction is almost agnostic

to whether or not the nose and mouth are open.

Okay, so if I understand correctly,

there’s no reason to suspect that there are particular,

perhaps even non-overlapping sets of neurons

in pre-Buttsinger area of the brainstem

that trigger nasal versus mouth inhales?

No.

I would say that it’s not that the pre-Buttsinger complex

is not concerned and cannot influence that,

but it does not appear as if there’s any modulation

of whether or not it’s where the air is coming from,

whether it’s coming through your nasal passages

or through your mouth.

Great, thank you.

And then the other question I have

is that these intercostal muscles between the ribs

that move the ribs up and out, if I understand correctly,

and the diaphragm, are those skeletal,

or as the Brits would say, skeletal muscles,

or smooth muscles?

What type of muscle are we talking about here?

As I said earlier, these are skeletal,

I didn’t say they were skeletal muscles,

but they’re muscles that need neural input

in order to move.

You talked about smooth muscles.

They’re specialized muscles like we have in the gut

and in the heart, and these are muscles

that are capable of actually contracting

and relaxing on their own.

So the heart beats, it doesn’t need neural input

in order to beat.

The neural inputs modulate the strength of it

and the frequency, but they beat on their own.

The skeletal muscles involved in breathing

need neural input.

Now, there are smooth muscles that have an influence

on breathing, and these are muscles

that are lining the airways.

And so the airways have smooth muscle,

and when they become activated,

the smooth muscle can contract or relax.

And when they contract inappropriately

is when you have problems breathing like an asthma.

Asthma is a condition where you get

inappropriate constriction of the smooth muscles

of the airways.

So there’s no reason to think that in asthma

that the pre-Buttsinger or these other neuronal centers

in the brain that activate breathing,

that they are involved or causal for things like asthma.

As of now, I would say the preponderance of evidence

is that it’s not involved,

but we’ve not really fully investigated that.

Thank you.

Sorry to break your flow,

but I was terribly interested in knowing answers

to those questions, and you provided them, so thank you.

Now, remind me again where I was in my-

We were just landing in pre-Buttsinger,

and we will return to the naming of pre-Buttsinger

because it’s a wonderful and important story, really,

that I think people should be aware of.

But maybe you could march us through the brain centers

that you’ve discovered and others have worked on as well

that control breathing, pre-Buttsinger

as well as related structures.

So when we discovered the pre-Buttsinger,

we thought that it was the primary source

of all rhythmic respiratory movements,

both inspiration and expiration.

The notion of a single source is like day or night.

It’s like they’re all coming,

they all have the same origin,

that the earth rotates and day follows night.

And we thought that the pre-Buttsinger complex

would be inhalation, exhalation.

And then in a series of experiments

we did in the early part of 2000,

we discovered that there seemed to be another region

which was dominant in producing expiratory movements,

that is the exhalation.

We had made a fundamental mistake

with the discovery of the pre-Buttsinger,

not taking into account that at rest,

expiratory muscle activity or exhalation is passive.

So if that’s the case, a group of neurons

that might generate active expiration,

that is to contract the expiratory muscles

like the abdominal muscles or the internal intercostals,

are just silent.

We just thought it wasn’t there,

it was coming from one place.

But we got evidence that in fact

it may have been coming from another place.

And following up on these experiments,

we discovered that there was a second oscillator

and that oscillator is involved in generating

what we call active expiration,

that is this active, like a shh,

or when you begin to exercise,

you have to go, shh, shh, shh, shh,

and actually move that air out.

This group of cells, which is silent at rest,

suddenly becomes active to drive those muscles.

And it appears that it’s an independent oscillator.

Maybe you could just clarify for people

what an oscillator is.

Okay, an oscillator is something that goes in a cycle.

So you can have a pendulum as an oscillator

going back and forth.

The Earth is an oscillator because it goes around

and it’s day and night.

Some people’s moods are oscillating.

Oscillating.

And it depends how regular they are.

You can have oscillators that are highly regular

that are in a watch,

or you can have those that are sporadic or episodic.

Breathing is one of those oscillators

that for life has to be working continuously, 24-7.

It starts late in the third trimester

because it has to be working when you’re born.

And basically works throughout life.

And if it stops, if there’s no intervention

beyond a few minutes, it will likely be fatal.

What is this second oscillator called?

Well, we found it in a region around the facial nucleus.

So we initially, when this region was initially identified,

we thought it was involved in sensing carbon dioxide.

It was what we call a central chemoreceptor.

That is, we want to keep carbon dioxide levels,

particularly in the brain, at a relatively stable level

because the brain is extraordinarily sensitive

to changes in pH.

If there’s a big shift in carbon dioxide,

there’ll be a big shift in brain pH

and that’ll throw your brain,

if I can use the technical term, out of whack.

And so you want to regulate that.

And the way to regulate something in the brain

is you have a sensor in the brain.

And others basically identified

that the ventral surface of the brainstem,

that is the part of the brainstem that’s on this side,

was critical for that.

And then we identified a structure

that was near the trapezoid nucleus.

It was not named in any of these neuroanatomical atlases.

So we just picked a name out of the hat

and we called it the retrotrapezoid nucleus.

I should clarify for people,

when Jack is saying trapezoid,

he doesn’t mean the trapezoid muscles.

Trapezoid refers to the shape of this nucleus,

this cluster of neurons.

Parafacial makes me think that this general area

is involved in something related to mouth or face.

Is it an area rich with neurons

controlling other parts of the face,

eye blinks, nose twitches, lip curls, lip smacks?

If you go back in an evolutionary sense,

and a lot of things that are hard to figure out

begin to make sense when you look at the evolution

of the nervous system,

when control of facial muscles,

going back to more primitive creatures,

because they had to take things in their mouth for eating,

so we call that the face sort of developed.

The eyes were there, the mouth is there.

These nuclei that contained the motor neurons,

a lot of the control systems for them

developed in the immediate vicinity.

So if you think about the face,

there’s a lot of sub-nuclei around there

that had various roles

at various different times in evolution.

And at one point in evolution,

the facial muscles were probably very important

in moving fluid in and out of the mouth

and moving air in and out of the mouth.

And so part of these many different sub-nuclei

now seems to be in mammals

to be involved in the control of expiratory muscles.

But we have to remember that mammals are very special

when it comes to breathing,

because we’re the only class of vertebrates

that have a diaphragm.

If you look at amphibians and reptiles,

they don’t have a diaphragm.

And the way they breathe

is not by actively inspiring and passively expiring.

They breathe by actively expiring and passively inspiring

because they don’t have a powerful inspiratory muscle.

And somewhere along the line, the diaphragm developed.

And there are lots of theories about how it developed.

I don’t think it’s particularly clear.

There was something you can find in alligators and lizards.

That could have turned into a muscle that was the diaphragm.

The amazing thing about the diaphragm

is that it’s mechanically extremely efficient.

And what do I mean by that?

Well, if you look at how oxygen gets from outside the body

into the bloodstream,

the critical passage is across the membrane in the lung.

It’s called the alveolar capillary membrane.

The alveolus is part of the lung

and the blood runs through capillaries,

which are the smallest tubes in the circulatory system.

And at that point, oxygen can go from the air-filled alveolus

into the blood.

Which is amazing.

I find that amazing.

Even though it’s just purely mechanical,

the idea that we have these little sacs in our lungs,

we inhale and the air goes in

and literally the oxygen can pass into the bloodstream.

Passes into the bloodstream.

But the rate at which it passes

will depend on the characteristics of the membrane,

what the distance is between the alveolus

and the blood vessel, the capillary.

But the key element is the surface area.

The bigger the surface area,

the more oxygen that can pass through.

It’s entirely a passive process.

There’s no magic about making oxygen go in.

Now, how do you get a pack,

a large surface area in a small chest?

Well, you start out with one tube, which is the trachea.

The trachea expands.

Now you have two tubes.

Then you have four tubes and it keeps branching.

At some point, at the end of those branches,

you put a little sphere, which is an alveolus.

And that determines what the surface area is going to be.

Now, you then have a mechanical problem.

You have this surface area.

You have to be able to pull it apart.

So imagine you have a little square of elastic membrane.

It doesn’t take a lot of force to pull it apart.

But now if you increase it by 50 times,

you need a lot more force to pull it apart.

So amphibians, who were breathing

not by compressing the lungs

and then just passively expanding it,

weren’t able to generate a lot of force.

So they have relatively few branches.

So if you look at the surface area

that they pack in their lungs,

relative to their body size, it’s not very impressive.

Whereas when you get to mammals,

the amount of branching that you have

is you have four to 500 million alveoli.

How, if we were to take those four to 5 million alveoli?

A hundred million, four to five, a hundred million.

A hundred million, excuse me.

And lay those out flat,

what sort of surface area are we talking about?

About 70 square meters,

which is about a third the size of a tennis court.

Wow.

So you have a membrane inside of you,

a third the size of a tennis court

that you actually have to expand every breath.

And you do that without exerting much of a,

you don’t feel it.

And that’s because you have this amazing muscle,

the diaphragm, which because of its positioning,

just by moving two thirds of an inch down

is able to expand that membrane enough

to move air into the lungs.

Now, at rest, the value of air in your lungs

is about two and a half liters.

Do we need to convert that to quarts?

No.

It’s about two and a half liters.

When you take a breath,

you’re taking another 500 milliliters or half a liter.

That’s the size maybe of my fist.

So you’re increasing the volume by 20%.

But you’re doing that by pulling

on this 70 square meter membrane.

But that’s enough to bring enough fresh air into the lung

to mix in with the air that’s already there

that the oxygen levels in your bloodstream

goes from a partial pressure of oxygen,

which is 40 millimeters of mercury

to a hundred millimeters of mercury.

So that’s a huge increase in oxygen

and that’s enough to sustain normal metabolism.

So we have this amazing mechanical advantage

by having a diaphragm.

Do you think that our brains are larger than that

of other mammals in part

because of the amount of oxygen

that we have been able to bring into our system?

I would say a key step in the ability

to develop a large brain

that has a continuous demand for oxygen is the diaphragm.

Without a diaphragm, you’re an amphibian.

And there’s another solution to increasing oxygen uptake,

which is the way birds breathe,

but birds have other limitations

and they still can’t get brains as big as mammals have.

So the brain utilizes maybe 20%

of all the oxygen that we intake

and it needs it continuously.

The brain doesn’t want to be neglected.

So this puts certain demands on the breathing system.

In other words, you can’t shut it down for a while,

which poses other issues.

You’re born and you have to mature.

You have the small body, you have a small lung,

you have a very pliant rib cage,

and now you have to develop into an adult,

which has a stiffer rib cage.

And so there are changes happening

in your brain and your body

where the neural control of breathing

has to change on the fly.

It’s not like for things like vision,

where you have the opportunity to sleep

and while you’re sleeping,

the brain is capable of doing things

that are not easy to do during wakefulness,

like the construction crew comes in during sleep.

The change in breathing have been described

as trying to build an airplane while it’s flying.

Basically what Jack is saying

is that respiration science is more complex

and hardworking than vision science,

which is a direct jab at me

that some of you might’ve missed,

but I definitely did not miss.

And I appreciate that you always take the opportunity

like a good New Yorker

to give me a good healthy intellectual jab.

A question related to diaphragmatic breathing

versus non-diaphragmatic breathing,

because the way you describe it,

the diaphragm is always involved,

but over the years,

whether it be for yoga class or a breath work thing,

or you hear online that we should be breathing

with our diaphragm,

that rather than lifting our rib cage when we breathe

and our chest, that it is healthier, in air quotes,

or better somehow to have the belly expand when we inhale.

I’m not aware of any particular studies

that have really examined the direct health benefits

of diaphragmatic versus non-diaphragmatic breathing,

but if you don’t mind commenting

on anything you’re aware of

as it relates to diaphragmatic

versus non-diaphragmatic breathing,

whether or not people tend to be diaphragmatic breathers

by default, et cetera,

that would be, I think, interesting to a number of people.

Well, I think by default,

we are obligate diaphragm breathers.

There may be pathologies where the diaphragm is compromised

and you have to use other muscles,

and that’s a challenge.

Certainly, at rest,

other muscles can take over,

but if you need to increase your ventilation,

the diaphragm is very important.

It would be hard to increase your ventilation otherwise.

Do you pay attention to whether or not

you are breathing in a manner

where your belly goes out a little bit as you inhale?

Because I can do it both ways, right?

I can inhale, bring my belly in,

or I can inhale, push my diaphragm and belly out,

not the diaphragm out,

but, and that’s interesting, right?

Because it’s a completely different muscle set

for each version.

Well, in the context of things like breath practice,

I’m a bit agnostic about the effects

of some of the different patterns of breathing.

Clearly, some are gonna work through different mechanisms,

and we can talk about that,

but at certain level, for example,

whether it’s primarily diaphragm

where you move your abdomen or not,

I am agnostic about it.

I think that the changes that breathing induces

in emotion and cognition,

I have different ideas about what the influence is,

and I don’t see that primarily

as how which particular muscles you’re choosing,

but that just could be my own prejudice.

Okay, and we will return to that

as a general theme in a little bit.

I wanna ask you about sighing.

One of the many great gifts

that you’ve given us over the years

is an understanding of these things

that we call physiological sighs.

Could you tell us about physiological sighs,

what’s known about them,

what your particular interest in them is,

and what they’re good for?

Very interesting and important question.

So everyone has a sense of what a sigh is.

We certainly, when we’re emotional in some ways,

we’re stressed, we’re particularly happy,

we’ll take a, we’ll sigh.

It turns out that we’re sighing all the time,

and when I would ask people

who are not particularly knowledgeable

that haven’t read my papers or James Nestor’s book

listen to your podcast,

they’re usually off by two orders of magnitude

about how frequently we sigh on the low side.

In other words, they say once an hour,

10 times a day.

We sigh about every five minutes,

and I would encourage anyone who finds that

to be a unbelievable fact

is to lie down in a quiet room

and just breathe normally,

just relax, just let go,

and just pay attention to your breathing,

and you’ll find that every couple of minutes

you’re taking a deep breath,

and you can’t stop it.

You know, it just happens.

Now why?

Well, we have to go back to the lung again.

The lung has these 500 million alveoli,

and they’re very tiny.

They’re 200 microns across.

So they’re really, really tiny,

and you can think of them as fluid-filled.

They’re fluid-lined,

and the reason they’re fluid-lined

has to do with the esoterica

of the mechanics of that.

It makes it a little easier to stretch them

with this fluid line,

which is called surfactant,

and surfactant is important during development.

It is a determining factor

when premature infants are born.

If they do not have lung surfactant,

it makes it much more challenging

to take care of them

than after they have lung surfactant,

which is sometime, if I remember correctly,

in the late second, early third trimester,

which it appears.

In any case, it’s fluid-lined.

Now think of a balloon that you would blow up,

but now before you blow it up,

fill the balloon with water.

Squeeze all the water out,

and now when you squeeze all the water out,

you notice the size of the balloons stick to each other.

Why is that?

Well, that’s because water has what’s called surface tension,

and when you have two surfaces of water together,

they actually tend to stick to each other.

Now when you try and blow that balloon up,

you know that it,

or you’ll notice if you’ve ever done it before,

that the balloon is a little harder to inflate

than if it were dry on the inside.

Why is that?

Because you have to overcome that surface tension.

Well, your alveoli have a tendency to collapse.

There’s 500 million of them.

They’re not collapsing at a very high rate,

but it’s a slow rate that’s not trivial.

And when an alveolus collapses,

it no longer can receive oxygen or take carbon dioxide out.

It’s sort of taken out of the equation.

Now if you have 500 million of them and you lose 10,

no big deal.

But if they keep collapsing,

you can lose a significant part

of the surface area of your lung.

Now, a normal breath is not enough to pop them open.

But if you take a deep breath,

it pops them open.

Through nose or mouth.

Doesn’t matter, doesn’t matter.

Or?

Just increase that lung volume,

because you’re just pulling on the lungs.

They’ll pop open about every five minutes.

And so we’re doing it every five minutes

in order to maintain the health of our lung.

In the early days of mechanical ventilation,

which was used to treat polio victims,

who had weakness of their respiratory muscles,

they’d be put in these big steel tubes.

And the way they would work

is that the pressure outside the body would drop.

That would put a expansion pressure on the lungs.

Excuse me, on the ribcage.

The ribcage would expand and then the lung would expand.

And then the pressure would go back to normal

and the lung and ribcage would go back to normal.

This was great for getting ventilation,

but there was a relatively high mortality rate.

It was a bit of a mystery.

And one solution was to just give bigger breaths.

They gave bigger breaths and the mortality rate dropped.

And it wasn’t until, I think it was the 50s,

where they realized that they didn’t have to increase

every breath to be big.

What they needed to do is every so often

they need to have one big breath.

So they gave a couple of minutes of normal breaths

and then one big breath,

just mimicking the physiological size.

And there the mortality rate dropped significantly.

And if you see someone on a ventilator in the hospital,

if you watch, every couple of minutes

that you see the membrane move up and down,

every couple of minutes, there’ll be a super breath

and that pops it open.

So there are these mechanisms for these physiological size.

So just like with the collapse of the lungs

where you need a big pressure to pop it open,

it’s the same thing with the alveoli.

You need a bigger pressure

and a normal breath is not enough.

So you have to take a big inhale.

And what nature has done is instead of requiring us

to remember to do it, it does it automatically.

And it does it about every five minutes.

And one of the questions we asked is how is this happening?

Why every five minutes?

What’s doing it?

And we got into it through a backdoor.

Typical of the way a lot of science gets done,

there’s a serendipitous event where you run across a paper

and something clicks and you just, you follow it up.

Sometimes you go down blind ends,

but this turned out to be incredibly productive.

One of the guys in my lab was reading a paper about stress

and during stress, lots of things happen in the body.

One of which is that the hypothalamus,

which is very reactive to body state,

releases peptides, which are specialized molecules,

which then circulate throughout the brain and body

to have particular effects,

usually to help deal better with the stress.

And one class of the peptides that are released

are called Bomberson-related peptides.

And he also realized, because he was a breathing guy,

that when you’re stressed, you sigh more.

So we said, all right, maybe they’re related.

Bomberson is relatively cheap to buy.

We said, let’s buy some Bomberson,

throw it in the brainstem, let’s see what happens.

And one of the nice things about some experiments

that we try to design is to fail quickly.

So here we had the idea, we throw Bomberson in

and if Bomberson did nothing,

nothing lost, maybe $50 to buy the Bomberson.

But if it did something, it might be of some interest.

One afternoon, he did the experiment.

And he comes to me, he says,

I won’t quote exactly what he said

because it might need to be censored,

but he said, look at this.

And it was in a rat.

Rats sigh about every two minutes.

They’re smaller than we are

and they need to sigh more often.

Their sigh rate went from 20 to 30 per hour

to 500 per hour when he put Bomberson

into the prebutzinger complex.

Amazing.

And the way he did that is he took

a very, very fine glass needle and anesthetized a rat

and inserted that needle directly

into the prebutzinger complex.

So it wasn’t a generalized delivery of the peptide,

it was localized to the prebutzinger

and the sigh rate went through the roof.

And I would add that that was an important experiment

to deliver the Bomberson directly to that site

because one could have concluded

that the injection of the Bomberson increased sighing

because it increased stress

rather than directly increased sighing.

Amongst hundreds of other possible interpretations.

So the precision here is very important

and that goes back to what I said at the very beginning.

Knowing where this is happening

allows you to do the proper investigations.

If we didn’t know where the inspiratory rhythm

was originating, we never could have done this experiment.

And so then we did another experiment.

We said, okay, what happens if we take the cells

in the prebutzinger that are responding to the peptides?

So neurons will respond to a peptide

because they have specialized receptors for that peptide.

And not every neuron expresses those receptors.

In the prebutzinger complex,

it’s probably a few hundred out of thousands.

So we used the technique we had used before.

And this is a technique developed by Doug Lappy

down in San Diego,

where you could take a peptide

and conjugate it with a molecule called saprin.

Saprin is a plant-derived molecule

which is a cousin to ricin.

And many of your listeners may have heard of ricin.

It’s a ribosomal toxin.

It’s very nasty.

A single stab with an umbrella will kill you,

which is something that supposedly happened

to a Bulgarian diplomat by a Russian operative

on a bridge in London.

He got stabbed.

And the way ricin works is it goes inside a cell,

crosses the cell membrane, goes inside the cell,

kills the cell, then it goes to the next cell,

and then the next cell, and then the next cell.

It’s extremely dangerous.

In fact, it’s firstly impossible to work on in a lab

in the United States.

They won’t let you touch it.

Ricin.

Because we’ve worked with saprin many times.

Saprin is safe because it doesn’t cross cell membranes.

So you get an injection of saprin,

it won’t do anything because it stays outside of cells.

Please, nobody do that,

even though it doesn’t cross cell membranes.

Please, nobody inject saprin,

whether or not you are a operative or otherwise.

Thank you, Andrew, for protecting me there.

But what Doug Lappi figured out

is that when a ligand binds to a receptor,

that is, when a molecule binds to its receptor,

in many cases, that receptor-ligand complex

gets pulled inside the cell.

So it goes from the membrane of the cell inside the cell.

Sure, like you can’t go to the dance alone,

but if you’re coupled up, you get in the door.

That’s right.

So what he figured out is he put saprin to the peptide,

the peptide binds to its receptor, it gets internalized,

and then when it’s inside the cell,

saprin does the same thing that ricin does.

It kills the cell.

But then it can’t go into the next cell.

So the only cells that get killed,

or the more polite term, ablated,

are cells that express that receptor.

So if you have a big conglomeration of cells,

you have a few thousand,

and only 50 of them express that receptor,

then you inject the saprin conjugated to the ligand,

to the peptide, and only those 50 cells die.

So we took bombicin conjugated to saprin,

injected in the prebiotic complex of rats,

and it took about a couple of days

for the saprin to actually ablate cells.

And what happened is that the mice started sighing less

and less, excuse me, the rats started sighing less

and less and less and less, and essentially stopped sighing.

So your student, or postdoc, was it,

murdered these cells, and as a consequence,

the sighing goes away.

Correct.

What was the consequence of eliminating sighing

on the internal state or the behavior of the rats?

In other words, if one can’t sigh,

generate physiological sighs,

what is the consequence on state of mind?

You would imagine that carbon dioxide

would build up more readily

or to higher levels in the bloodstream,

and that the animals would be more stressed.

That’s the kind of logical extension

of the way we set it up.

It was less benign than that.

When the animals got to the point

where they weren’t sighing,

then, and we did not determine this,

but the presumption was that their lung function

significantly deteriorated,

and their whole health deteriorated significantly,

and we had to sacrifice them.

So I can’t tell you whether they were stressed or not,

but their breathing got to be significantly

deteriorated that we sacrificed them at that point.

Now, we don’t know whether that is specifically related

to the fact they didn’t sigh,

or that there was secondary damage

due to the fact that some cells die,

so we never determined that.

Now, after we did this study,

to be candid, it wasn’t a high priority for us

to get this out the door and publish it.

So it stayed on the shelf.

And then I got a phone call

from a graduate student at Stanford, Kevin Yackel,

who starts asking me all these interesting questions

about breathing, and I’m happy to answer them,

but at some point, it concerned me

because he was working for a renowned biochemist

who worked on lung and Drosophila fruit flies,

Mark Krasnow.

Yeah, my next-door colleague.

Right, and I said, why are you asking me this?

And he said, I was an undergraduate at UCLA,

and you gave a lecture in my undergraduate class,

and I was curious about breathing ever since.

So that’s one of those things which, as a professor,

you love to hear, that actually is something

you really affected the life of a student.

Well, and you birthed a competitor,

but you had only yourself to blame.

I don’t look at that as a competitor.

I think that there’s enough interesting things to go on.

I know some of our neuroscience colleagues say,

you can work in my lab, but then when you leave my lab,

you got to work on something different.

No one I ever trained with said that.

It’s open field.

You want to work on something, you hop in the mix.

But there are people like that, neuroscientists like that.

I never felt like that.

I hear that their breathing apparati are disrupted,

and it causes a brain dysfunction

that leads to the behavior you just described.

That’s actually not true, but in terms of the,

so before we talk about the beautiful story

with Yackel and Krasnow and Feld lab,

I want to just make sure that I understand.

So if physiological size don’t happen,

basically breathing overall suffers.

Well, that would go back to the observations

in polio victims in these high end lungs

where the principal deficit was there was no hyperinflation

of the lungs, and many of them deteriorated and died.

And just to stay on this one more moment

before we move to what you were about to describe,

we hear often that people will overdose

on drugs of various kinds because they stop breathing.

So barbiturates, alcohol combined with barbiturates

is a common cause of death for drug users

and contraindications of drugs and these kinds of things.

You hear all the time about celebrities dying

because they combined alcohol with barbiturates.

Is there any evidence that the size that occur during sleep

or during states of deep, deep relaxation and sedation

that size recover the brain?

Because you could imagine that if the size don’t happen

as a consequence of some drug impacting these brain centers,

that that could be one cause

of basically asphyxiation and death.

If you look at the progression of any mammal to death,

you find that their breathing slows down,

a death due to, quote, natural causes.

Their breathing slows down, it will stop,

and then they’ll gasp.

So we have the phrase dying gasp,

the super large breaths.

They’re often described as an attempt to auto-resuscitate.

That is, you take that super deep breath

and that maybe it can kickstart the engine again.

We do not know the degree to such things as gasp

are really size that are particularly large.

And so if you suppress the ability to gasp

in an individual who is subject to an overdose,

then whereas they might have been able

to re-arouse their breathing,

if that’s prevented, they don’t get re-aroused.

So that is certainly a possibility.

But this has not been investigated.

I mean, one of the things that I’m interested in

is in individuals who have

diseases which will affect pre-Butzinger complex.

And there’s data in Parkinson’s disease

and multiple system atrophy,

which is another form of neurodegeneration,

where there’s loss of neurons in pre-Butzinger.

And the question is, and it also may happen in ALS,

sometimes referred to as Lou Gehrig’s disease,

amyotrophic lateral sclerosis.

These individuals often die during sleep.

We have an idea that we have not been able

to get anyone to test,

is that patients with Parkinson’s,

patients with MLS,

typically breathe normally during wakefulness.

The disturbances that they have in breathing

is during sleep.

So Parkinson’s patients at the end stages of the disease

often have significant disturbances in their sleep pattern,

but not during wakefulness.

And we think that what could be happening

is that the approximate cause of death

is not heart failure,

is that they become apnea,

they stop breathing and don’t resuscitate.

And that resuscitation may or may not be due

to an explicit suppression of size,

but to an overall suppression of the whole apparatus

of the pre-Butzinger complex.

Got it, thank you.

So Yackel calls you up.

So he calls me up and he’s great kid,

super smart.

And he tells me about these experiments that he’s doing,

where he’s looking in a database

to try and find out what molecules are enriched

in regions of the brain that are critical for breathing.

So we and others have mapped out these regions

in the brain stem.

And he was looking at one of these databases

to see what’s enriched.

And I said, that’s great.

Will you be willing to sort of share work together?

He says, no, my advisor doesn’t want me to do that.

So I said, okay.

But Kevin’s a great kid and I enjoyed talking to him

and he’s a smart guy.

And what I found in academia

is that the smartest people

only want to hire people smarter than them

and have the preference to interact

with people smarter than them.

The faculty who are not at the highest level

and at every institution, there’s a distribution.

There are ones above the mean and those below the mean.

Those who are below the mean are very threatened by that.

And I saw Kevin as like a shining light

and I didn’t care whether he was gonna out-compete me

because whatever he did was gonna help me in the field.

I did whatever I can to help, to work with Kevin.

So at one point, I got invited to give grand rounds

in neurology at Stanford.

Turns out an undergraduate student who had worked with me

was now head of the training program for neurologists

at Stanford and he invited me.

And at the end of my visit, I go to Mark Krasnow’s office

and Kevin is there and a postdoc, Pungley,

who was also working on a project was there.

And towards the end of the conversation,

the, Mark says to me, you know, we found this one molecule

which is highly concentrated

in an important region for breathing.

I said, oh, that’s great, what is it?

And he says, I can’t tell you because we wanna work on it.

So, of course I’m disappointed, but I realized that

the ethic in some areas of science

or the custom in some areas of science

is that until you get a publication,

you be relatively restricted in sharing the information.

Mark and I are gonna have a chat when I get back.

Well, he may remember the story differently,

but I said, okay.

And as I’m walking out the door,

I remember these experiments I described to you

about bombosin and that was the only unusual molecule

we’re working on.

So, the reason I’m rushing out the door

is I have a flight to catch.

So, I stick my head in and I said,

is this molecule related to bombosin?

And then I run off, I don’t even wait for them to reply.

I get to the airport, Mark calls me and he says,

bombosin, the peptide we found is related to bombosin,

what does it do?

And I said, I’m not telling.

Oh my, I’m so glad I wasn’t involved in this collaboration.

No, no, but that was sort of a tease

because I said, well, let’s work together on this.

And then we worked together on this.

It was a prisoner’s dilemma at that point.

So, Kevin Yackel is spectacular, has his own lab at UCSF.

And the work that I’m familiar with from Kevin

is worth mentioning now, or I’ll ask you to mention it,

which is this reciprocal relationship between brain state,

or we could even say emotional state and breathing.

And I’d love to get your thoughts on how breathing interacts

with other things in the brain.

You’ve beautifully described how breathing controls

the lungs, the diaphragm, and the interactions

between oxygen and carbon dioxide and so forth.

But as we know, when we get stressed,

our breathing changes.

When we’re happy and relaxed, our breathing changes.

But also, if we change our breathing,

we, in some sense, can adjust our internal state.

What is the relationship between brain state and breathing?

And if you would, because I know you have a particular love

of one particular aspect of this,

what is the relationship between brain rhythms,

oscillations, if you will, and breathing?

This is a topic which has really intrigued me

over the past decade.

I would say before that, I was in my silo,

just interested about how the rhythm of breathing

is generated, and didn’t really pay much attention

to this other stuff.

For some reason, I got interested in it,

and I think it was triggered by an article

in the New York Times about mindfulness.

Now, believe it or not, although I’d lived in California

for 20 years at that time, I never heard of mindfulness.

It’s staggering how isolated you can be from the real world.

And I Googled it, and there was a mindfulness institute

at UCLA, and they were giving courses in meditation.

So I said, oh, this is great, because I can now see

whether or not the breathing part of meditation

has anything to do with the purported effects

of meditation.

So I signed up for the course,

and as I joked to you before, I had two goals.

One was to see whether or not breathing had an effect,

and the other was to levitate.

Because I grew up in all these Kung Fu things,

and all the monks could levitate when they meditated,

so why not?

You know, we have a model in the lab,

you can’t do anything interesting

if you’re afraid of failing.

And if I fail to levitate, well, at least I tried.

And I should tell you now, I still haven’t done it yet,

but I haven’t given up yet.

I haven’t given up.

So I took this course in mindfulness,

and it became apparent to me

that the breathing part was actually critical.

It wasn’t simply a distraction or a focus.

You know, they could have had you move your index finger

to the same effect, but I really believed

that the breathing part was involved.

Now, I’m not an unbiased observer,

so the question is, how can I demonstrate that?

I didn’t feel competent to do experiments in humans,

and I didn’t feel I could design

the right experiments in humans,

but I felt, maybe I can study this in rodents.

So we got this idea

that we’re gonna teach rodents to meditate.

And, you know, that’s laughable,

but we said, but if we can,

then we can actually study how this happens.

So believe it or not, I was able to get

a sort of a starter grant, an R21 from NCCIH.

That’s the National Complementary Medicine Institute.

A wonderful institute, I should mention.

Our government puts major tax dollars

toward studies of things like meditation, breath work,

supplements, herbs, acupuncture.

This is, I think, not well-known,

and it’s an incredible thing that our government does that,

and I think it deserves a nod and more funding.

I totally agree with you.

I think that it’s the kind of thing

that many of us, including many scientists,

thinks is too woo-woo and unsubstantiated,

but we’re learning more and more.

You know, we used to laugh at neuroimmunology,

that the nervous system didn’t have anything

to do with the immune system,

and pain itself can influence your immune system.

I mean, there are all these things that we’re learning

that we used to dismiss,

and I think there’s real nuggets to be learned here.

So they went out on a limb

and they funded this particular project,

and now I’m gonna leap ahead,

because for three years,

we threw stuff up against the wall that didn’t work,

and recently, we had a major breakthrough.

We found a protocol by which we can get mice

to breathe slowly, awake mice to breathe slowly.

I won’t tell you.

Normally, they don’t breathe slowly.

No, no, in other words, whatever their normal breath is,

we could slow it down by a factor of 10,

and they’re fine doing that.

So we could do that for, we did that 30 minutes a day

for four weeks, okay, like a breath practice.

Do they levitate?

We haven’t measured that yet, I would say.

A priori, we haven’t seen them floating

to the top of their cage, but we haven’t weighed them.

Maybe they weigh less.

Maybe levitation is graded,

and so maybe if you weigh less,

it’s sort of partial levitation.

In any case, we then tested them,

and we had control animals, mice,

where we did everything the same

except the manipulation we made

did not slow down their breathing,

but they went through everything else.

We then put them through a standard fear conditioning,

which we did with my colleague, Michael Fanzelow,

who’s one of the real gurus of fear,

and we measured a standard test

is to put mice in a condition

where they’re concerned that receive a shock,

and their response is that they freeze,

and a measure of how fearful they are

is how long they freeze.

This is well-validated, and it’s way above my pay grade

to describe the validity of the test, but it’s very valid.

The control mice had a freezing time

which was just the same as ordinary mice would have.

The ones that went through our protocol

froze much, much less.

According to Michael, the degree to which

they showed less freezing was as much

as if there was a major manipulation in the amygdala,

which is a part of the brain

that’s important in fear processing.

It’s a staggering change.

The problem we have now is the grant ran out of money,

the postdoc working on it left,

and now we have to try and piece together everything,

but the data is spectacular.

Well, I think it’s, I’ll just pause you for a moment there

because I think that the, you know,

you’re talking about a rodent study,

but I think the benefits of doing rodent studies

that you can get deep into mechanism,

and for people that might think,

well, we’ve known that meditation has these benefits,

why do you need to get mechanistic science?

I think that one thing that’s important

for people to remember is that, first of all,

as many people as one might think are meditating out there

or doing breath work,

a far, far, far greater number of people are not, right?

I mean, there’s a, the majority of people

don’t take any time to do dedicated breath work

nor meditate, so whatever can incentivize people

would be wonderful,

but the other thing is that it’s never really been clear

to me just how much meditation is required

for a real effect, meaning a practical effect.

People say 30 minutes a day, 20 minutes a day,

once a week, twice a week, same thing with breath work.

Finding minimum or effective thresholds

for changing neural circuitry is what I think

is the holy grail of all these practices,

and that’s only going to be determined

by the sorts of mechanistic studies that you describe.

So this is wonderful.

I do hope the work gets completed,

and we can talk about ways that we can ensure

that that happens, but-

But let me add one thing to what you’re saying, Andrew.

One of the issues, I think, for a lot of people

is that there’s a placebo effect.

That is, in humans, they can respond to something

even though the mechanism has nothing to do

with what the intervention is,

and so it’s easy to say that the meditative response

has a big component, which is a placebo effect.

My mice don’t believe in the placebo effect,

and so if we could show there’s a bona fide effect in mice,

it is convincing in ways that,

no matter how many human experiments you did,

the control for the placebo effect

is extremely difficult in humans.

In mice, it’s a non-issue.

So I think that that, in and of itself,

would be an enormous message to send.

Excellent, and indeed, a better point.

I think a 30-minute-a-day meditation in these mice,

if I understand correctly, the meditation,

we don’t know what they’re thinking about.

Oh, it’s breath practice.

Right, so it’s breath practice,

because presumably they’re not thinking

about their third eye center,

lotus position, levitation, whatever it is.

They’re not instructed as to what to do,

and if they were, they probably wouldn’t do it anyway.

So 30 minutes a day

in which breathing is deliberately slowed,

or is slowed relative to their normal patterns of breathing.

Got it.

What was the frequency of sighing during that 30 minutes?

Unclear. We don’t know yet.

Well, no, we have the data.

We just, we’re analyzing the data.

To be determined, or to be announced at some point.

So the fear centers are altered in some way

that creates a shorter fear response to a foot shock.

Right.

What are some other examples that you are aware of

from work in your laboratory,

or work in other laboratories for that matter,

about interactions between breathing

and brain state or emotional state?

So this gets back to our prior conversation.

I sort of went off on that tangent.

We need, I think we need to think separately

of the effect of volitional changes of breathing

on emotion versus the effect,

the effect of brain state on breathing.

So the effect of brain state on breathing,

like when you’re stressed,

is an effect presumably originating in higher centers,

if I can use that term, affecting breathing.

It’s, the reciprocal is that when you change breathing,

it affects your emotional state.

I think of those two things as different,

that may ultimately be tied together.

So there’s a landmark paper published in the 50s

where they stimulated in the amygdala of cats,

and depending on where they stimulated,

they got profound changes in breathing.

There’s like every pattern of breathing

you could possibly imagine.

They found a site in the amygdala

which could produce that.

So there’s clearly a powerful descending effect

coming from the amygdala, which is a major site

for processing emotion, fear, stress, and whatnot

that can affect breathing.

And clearly we have volitional control over breathing.

So we have profound effects there.

Now, I should say about emotional control of breathing,

I need to segue into talking about locked-in syndrome.

Locked-in syndrome is a devastating lesion

that happens in a part of the brainstem

where signals to controlled muscles are transmitted.

So the fibers coming from your motor cortex

go down to this part of the brainstem,

which is called the ventral pons.

And if there’s a stroke there,

it can damage these pathways.

What happens in individuals who have locked-in syndrome

is they lose all volitional movement

except lateral movement of the eyes

and maybe the ability to blink.

The reason they’re able to still blink and move their eyes

is that those control centers are rostral,

closer to, are not interrupted.

In other words, the interruption is below that.

They continue to breathe

and the centers for breathing

don’t require that volitional command.

In any case, they’re below that.

So they’re fine.

So these people continue to breathe.

Normal intelligence, but they can’t move.

There’s a great book called

The Diving Bell and the Butterfly

about a young man who this happens to

and he describes his life.

And it’s a real testament to the human condition

that he does this.

It’s a remarkable book.

It’s a short book.

Did he write the book by blinking?

He did it by blinking to his caretaker.

It’s pretty amazing.

And there was a movie, which I’ve never seen,

with Javier Bardeen as the protagonist,

but the book I highly recommend anyone to read.

So I had colleagues studying an individual

who had locked-in syndrome

and this patient breathed very robotically,

totally consistent, very regular.

They gave the patient a low oxygen mixture to breathe.

Ventilation went up.

A CO2 mixture to breathe.

Ventilation went up.

So all the regulatory apparatus for breathing was there.

They asked the patient to hold his breath

or to breathe faster.

Nothing happened.

Just the patient recognized the command,

but couldn’t change it.

And all of a sudden,

the patient’s breathing changed considerably.

And they said to the patient, what happened?

They said, you told a joke and I laughed.

And they went back.

Whenever they told a joke that the patient found funny,

the patient’s breathing pattern changed.

And you know your breathing pattern when you laugh

is you inhale, you go, ha, ha, ha, ha.

But it’s also very distinctive.

We have some neuroscience colleagues who will go unnamed

who if you heard them laugh 50 yards away,

you know exactly who they are.

Yeah, well, I’ll name him.

Eric Kandel.

For one.

Has an inspiratory laugh.

He’s famous for a, as opposed to a, ha, ha.

Exactly, exactly.

So it’s very stereotyped,

but it’s maintained in these people

who lose volitional control of breathing.

So there’s an emotive component controlling your breathing,

which has nothing to do with your volitional control.

And it goes down to a different pathway

because it’s not disrupted by this locked-in syndrome.

If you look at motor control of the face,

we have the volitional control of the face,

but we also have emotional control of the face,

which most of us can’t control.

So when we look at another person,

we’re able to read a lot about what their emotional state is

and that’s a lot about how primates communicate,

humans communicate.

And you have people who are good deceivers,

probably used car salesmen, poker players,

or now poker players, you know, have tells,

but many of them now wear, you know, dark glasses

because a lot of the tells you blink or whatnot.

Pupil size is a tell.

Pupil size, pupil size is a tell,

which is an autonomic function,

not a skeletal muscle function,

but we have all these skeletal muscles

which we’re controlling, which give us away.

I’ve tried to get my imaging friends

to image some of the great actors

that we have in Los Angeles.

You mean brain imagers?

Brain imagers, I’m sorry.

No, that’s all right.

I mean, yeah, brain imagers,

because I think when I ask you to smile,

I could tell that you’re not happy,

that you’re smiling because I ask you to smile.

I think I just-

I think you’re about to crack a joke,

but we’re old friends, so, you know.

No, I’m not, that, you know,

when you see a picture, like at a birthday or whatnot,

and say cheese, you could tell

that at least half of the people are not happy

they’re saying cheese.

Whereas a great actor, when they’re able to dissemble

in the fact that they’re sad or they’re happy,

you believe it, they’re not faking it.

It’s like, that’s great acting.

And I don’t think everyone could do that.

I think that the individuals who are able to do that

have some connection to the parts

of their emotive control system

that the rest of us don’t have.

Maybe they develop it through training and maybe not,

but I think that this can be imaged.

So I would like to get one of these great actors

in a imager and have them go through that

and then get a normal person

and see whether or not they can emulate that.

And I think you’re gonna find big differences

in the way they control this emotive thing.

So this emotive control of the facial muscles,

I think is in large part similar

to the emotive control of breathing.

So there’s that emotive control

and there’s that volitional control,

and they’re different, they’re different.

Now, you asked me about the yackle stuff.

The yackle paper had to do with ascending,

that the effect of breathing on emotion.

What Kevin found was that there was a population

of neurons in the prebutzial complex

that were always looking to things

that are projecting ultimately emotive neurons.

He found the population of cells

that projected to locus coeruleus.

Locus coeruleus, excuse me,

is one of those places in the brain

that seem to go everywhere.

It’s like a sprinkler system.

Exactly, exactly.

And influence mood,

and you’ve had podcasts about this.

I mean, there’s a lot of stuff going on with the amygdala.

So, excuse me, the locus coeruleus.

So you get into the locus coeruleus,

you can now spray information

out throughout the entire brain.

He found specific cells that projected

from prebutzinger to locus coeruleus.

And that these cells are inspiratory modulated.

Now, it’s been known for a long time,

since the 60s,

that if you look in the locus coeruleus of cats

when they’re awake,

you find many neurons that have respiratory modulation.

No one paid much attention to it.

I mean, why bother?

Not why bother paying attention,

but why would the brain bother to have these inputs?

So what Kevin did with Lindsey Schwartz

and Leeshan Loslier is they killed,

ablated those cells going to locus coeruleus

from prebutzinger.

And the animals became calmer.

And their EEG levels changed in ways

that are indicative that they became calmer.

And as I recall, they didn’t just become calmer,

but they weren’t really capable of high arousal states.

They were kind of flat.

I don’t think we really pursued that in the paper.

And so we’d have to ask John Huguenin about that.

But I-

He’s on the other side of my lap,

so we’ll ask him.

But nonetheless,

that beautifully illustrates

how there is a bidirectional control, right?

Well, that’s a setting.

Emotion.

Well, no, the two stories of the locked-in syndrome

plus the Yackel paper

shows that emotional states influence breathing

and breathing influences emotional states.

But you mentioned inspiration,

which I always call inhalation,

but people will follow.

No, that’s fine.

Those are interchangeable.

People can follow that.

There’s some interesting papers from Noam Sobel’s group

and from a number of other groups

that as we inhale or right after we inhale,

the brain is actually more alert

and capable of storing information than during exhales,

which I find incredible,

but it also makes sense.

I’m able to see things far better when my eyes are open

than when my eyelids are closed for that matter.

Maybe.

I don’t doubt,

I mean, Noam’s work is great.

Let me backtrack a bit

because I want people to understand

that when we’re talking about breathing

affecting emotional cognitive state,

it’s not simply coming from pre-Butzinger.

There are at least,

well, there are several other sites,

and let me sort of,

I need to sort of go through that.

One is olfaction.

So when you’re breathing, normal breathing,

you’re inhaling and exhaling.

This is creating signals coming from the nasal mucosa

that is going back into the olfactory bulb

that’s respiratory modulated.

And the olfactory bulb has a profound influence

and projections through many parts of the brain.

So there’s a signal arising from this rhythmic moving

of air in and out of the nose

that’s going into the brain

that has contained in it a respiratory modulation.

So that signal is there.

The brain doesn’t have to be using it,

but when it’s discriminating odor and whatnot,

that’s riding on a oscillation,

which is respiratory related.

Another potential source is the vagus nerve.

The vagus nerve is a major nerve,

which is containing afferents from all of the viscera.

Afferents just being signals too, yeah.

Signals from the viscera.

It also has signals coming from the brainstem down,

which are called efferents,

but it’s getting major signals from the lung,

from the gut,

and this is going up into the brainstem.

So it’s there.

There are very powerful receptors in the lung

that are responding to the lung volume,

the lung stretch.

They’re baroreceptors.

Sorry, we have a number of them,

like the piezo receptors of this year’s Nobel Prize, yeah.

So they’re responding to the expansion

and relaxation of the lung.

And so if you record from the vagus nerve,

you’ll see that there’s a huge respiratory modulation

due to the mechanical changes in the lung.

Now, why that is of interest

is that for some forms of refractory depression,

electrostimulation of the vagus nerve

can provide tremendous relief.

Why this is the case still remains to be determined,

but it’s clear that signals in the vagus nerve,

at least artificial signals in the vagus nerve,

can have a positive effect on reducing depression.

So it’s not a leap to think that under normal circumstances

that that rhythm coming in from the vagus nerve

is playing a role in normal processing.

Okay, let me continue.

Calmed oxide and oxygen levels.

Now, under normal circumstances,

your oxygen levels are fine.

And unless you go to altitude,

they don’t really change very much.

But your CO2 levels can change quite a bit

with even a relatively small change

in your overall breathing.

That’s gonna change your pH level.

I have a colleague, Alicia Moret,

who is working with patients who are anxious,

and many of them hyperventilate.

And as a result of that hyperventilation,

their calmed oxide levels are low.

And she has developed a therapeutic treatment

where she trains these people to breathe slower

and to restore their CO2 levels back to normal,

and she gets relief in their anxiety.

So CO2 levels,

which are not gonna affect brain function

on a breath-by-breath level,

although it does fluctuate breath-by-breath,

but sort of as a continuous background,

can change.

And if it’s changed chronically,

we know that highly elevated levels of CO2

can produce panic attacks.

And we don’t know the degree to which it gets exacerbated

by people who have a panic attack,

the degree to which their ambient CO2 levels

are affecting their degree of discomfort.

What about people who tend to be too calm,

meaning they’re feeling sleepy,

they’re under-breathing as opposed to over-breathing?

Is there any knowledge

of what the status of CO2 is in their system?

I don’t know, which doesn’t mean there’s no knowledge,

but I’m unaware, but that’s blissfully unaware.

I have not looked at that literature, so I don’t know.

And I have a feeling, I mean, most people,

or excuse me, most of the scientific literature

around breathing in humans that I’m aware of

relates to these stressed states

because they’re a little bit easier to study in the lab,

whereas people feeling under-stimulated

or exhausted all the time,

it’s a complicated thing to measure.

I mean, you can do it, but it’s not as straightforward.

Well, CO2 is easy to measure.

But in terms of the sort of the measures

for feeling fatigue, they’re somewhat indirect,

whereas stress, we can get at pulse rates in HRV

and things of that sort.

Well, I imagine that these devices that we’re all wearing

will soon be able to measure,

well, now they can measure oxygen levels,

oxygen saturation.

Which is amazing.

Yeah, but oxygens will pretty much stay

above 90% unless there’s some pathology

or you go to altitude.

But CO2 levels vary quite a bit.

And in fact, because they vary, your body is so sensitive,

the control of breathing,

like how much you breathe per minute,

is determined in a very sensitive way by the CO2 level.

So even a small change in your CO2

will have a significant effect on your ventilation.

So this is another thing that not only changes

your ventilation, but affects your brain state.

Now, another thing that could affect

how breathing practice can affect your emotional state

is simply the descending command.

Because breathing practice involves

volitional control of your breathing.

And therefore, there’s a signal that’s originating

somewhere in your motor cortex.

That is not, of course, that’s gonna go down

to pre-Butzinger.

But it’s also gonna send off collaterals to other places.

Those collaterals could obviously influence

your emotional state.

So we have quite a few different potential sources.

None of them that are exclusive.

There’s an interesting paper which shows

that if you block nasal breathing,

you still see breathing-related oscillations in the brain.

And this is where I think the mechanism is occurring,

is that these breathing-related oscillations in the brain,

they are playing a role in signal processing.

And maybe, should I talk a little bit about the role

that oscillations may be playing in signal processing?

Definitely, but before you do,

I just want to ask you a intermediate question.

We’ve talked a lot about inhalation,

inspiration, and exhalation.

What about breath holds?

You know, in apnea, for instance,

people are holding their breath,

whether or not it’s conscious or unconscious,

they’re holding their breath.

What’s known about breath holds in terms of how

it might interact with brain state or oxygen, CO2?

And I’m particularly interested in how breath holds

with lungs empty versus breath holds with lungs full

might differ in terms of their impact on the brain.

I’m not aware of any studies on this,

looking at a mechanistic level,

but I find it really interesting.

And even if there are no studies,

I’d love it if you’d care to speculate.

Well, one of the breath practices that intrigue me

is where you basically hyperventilate for a minute

and then hold your breath for as long as you can.

Tummo style, Wim Hof style.

We call it in the laboratory,

because frankly, before Tummo and before Wim,

it was referred to as cyclic hyperventilation.

So it’s basically,

followed by a breath hold.

And that breath hold could be done

with lungs full or lungs empty.

So I had a long talk with some colleagues

about what they might think the underlying mechanisms are,

particularly for the breath hold.

And I certainly envision that there’s a component

with respect to the presence or absence of that rhythmicity

in your cortex, which is having effect.

But the other thing with the hyperventilation,

hypoventilation or the apnea,

is your CO2 levels are going from low to high.

Anytime you’re holding your breath.

Anytime you’re holding your breath, okay.

And those are gonna have a profound influence.

Now, I have to talk about episodic hypoxia.

Because there’s a lot of work going on,

particularly with Gordon Mitchell

at the University of Florida,

is doing some extraordinary work on episodic hypoxia.

So in the 80s, David Milhorn

did some really intriguing work.

If I ask you to hold your breath, excuse me,

if I gave you a low oxygen mixture for a couple of minutes,

your breathing level would go up.

Because you wanna have more oxygen.

You’re starving for air.

Yeah.

No, you’re starving for oxygen.

All right.

Okay.

And for a couple of minutes, you go up,

you can reach some steady state level.

Not so hypoxic that you can’t reach an equilibrium.

And then I give you room air again,

your ventilation quickly relaxes back down to normal.

If on the other hand, I gave you three minutes of hypoxia,

five minutes of normoxia,

three minutes of hypoxia,

five minutes of normoxia,

three minutes hypoxia, five minutes of normoxia.

Normoxia being normal.

Normal, normal air.

Your ventilation goes up, down, up, down, up, down, up, down.

After the last episode,

your breathing comes down

and doesn’t continue to come down,

but rises again and stays up for hours.

Okay.

This is well validated now.

This was originally done in animals,

but in humans all the time.

It seems to have profound benefit

on motor function and cognitive function.

In what direction?

Positive, positive.

I’ve often toyed with the idea of getting a 5%,

an 8% oxygen.

Don’t do this listeners.

Getting an 8% oxygen tank by my desk

when I’m writing a grant

and doing like in blue velvet

and going through the episodic hypoxia

to improve my cognitive function.

I certainly could use improvement when I’m writing grants.

But you could do this without the low oxygen.

I mean, you could do this through breath work, presumably.

It’s hard to lower your oxygen enough.

Okay.

We’re going in the experimental studies,

they typically use 8% oxygen.

It’s hard to hold your breath long enough.

And there is another difference here.

That is what’s happening to your CO2 levels.

When you hold your breath,

your oxygen levels are dropping,

your CO2 levels are going up.

When you’re doing episodic hypoxia,

your CO2 levels are going to stay pretty normal.

Of course, you’re still breathing.

It’s just the oxygen levels are gone.

So unlike normal conditions,

which you described before

where oxygen is relatively constant

and CO2 is fluctuating depending on emotional state

and activity and things of that sort,

in episodic hypoxia, CO2 is relatively constant,

but you’re varying the oxygen level

coming into the system quite a bit.

I would say it’s relatively,

I would say CO2 is relatively constant,

but it’s not going to go in a direction

which is going to be significantly far from normal.

Whereas when you’re holding your breath,

you’re going to become both hypoxic

and hypercapnic at the same time.

We should explain to people

what hypoxic and hypercapnic are

because we haven’t done that.

Hypoxic is just a technical term

for low levels of oxygen.

Hyper, or you could say hypoxic, low.

Hyper is high, so hyperoxia,

or hypocapnia, low CO2,

or hypercapnia, highest levels of CO2.

So when you’re in episodic hypoxia,

if anything, you’re going to become hypocapnic,

not hypercapnic,

and that could play an influence on this.

One example that I remember,

and Gordon will have to forgive me

if I’m misquoting this,

is they had a patient who had a stroke

and had weakness in ankle flexion.

That is, excuse me, ankle extension,

to extend the ankle.

And so they had the patient in a seat

where they could measure ankle extension,

and then they measured it,

and then they exposed the patient

to episodic hypoxia,

and they measured, again,

the strength of the ankle extension

when way up.

And so Gordon is looking at this,

they’re looking at this now

for spinal cord rehab.

And I imagine for all sorts

of neuromuscular performance,

it could be beneficial.

Gordon is looking into athletic performance.

We have a project,

which we haven’t been able

to push to the next level,

to do golf.

So I think-

Why golf?

Because you love golf.

Well, it’s because it’s motor performance,

coordination, so it’s not simply

running as fast as you can.

It’s coordination, it’s concentration,

it’s a whole variety of things.

And so the idea would be

to get a group of golfers

and give them their placebo controls.

They don’t know whether they’re breathing

a gas mixture,

which is just normal air,

or hypoxic gas mixture,

although they may be able

to figure it out based on their response.

Do it under controlled circumstances

that do it into a net,

measure their length of their drives,

their dispersion and whatnot,

and see what happens.

Look, if we could find

that this works for golfers,

forget about cognitive function,

we could sell this

for unbelievable amounts of money.

That sounds like a terrible idea.

By the way, I’m not serious

about selling it, but-

I know you’re joking.

Maybe people should know

that you are joking about that.

No, I think that anything

that can improve cognitive

and neuromuscular performance

is going to be of interest

for a wide range of both pathologic states

like injury, TBI, et cetera.

One of the most frequent questions I get

is about recovery from concussion

or traumatic brain injury.

A lot of people think sports,

they think football,

they think rugby,

they think hockey,

but if you look at the statistics

on traumatic brain injury,

most of it is construction workers,

car crashes, bicycle accidents.

I mean, the sports part of it

is a tiny, tiny minuscule fraction

of the total amount

of traumatic brain injury out there.

I think these protocols tested

in the context of golf

would be very interesting

because of the constraints

of the measures, as you mentioned,

and it could be exported

to a number of things.

I want to just try and imagine

whether or not there is any kind

of breathing pattern or breath work,

just to be direct about it,

that even partially mimics

what you described

in terms of episodic hypoxia.

I’ve done a lot of Tumor-Wim Hof

cyclic hyperventilation type breathing before.

My lab studies this in humans,

and what we find is that

if people do cyclic hyperventilation,

so for about a minute,

then exhale, hold their breath

for 15 to 60 seconds,

depending on what they can do,

and just keep repeating

that for about five minutes,

it seems to me that it at least

partially mimics the state

that you’re talking about

because afterwards,

people report heightened levels

of alertness, lower levels

of kind of triggering

due to stressful events.

They feel comfortable

at a higher level

of autonomic arousal, cognitive focus,

a number of improvements

that are pretty impressive

that any practitioner

of Wim Hof or Tumor

will be familiar with.

Is that pattern of breathing even,

can we say that it maps

to what you’re describing

in some general sense?

Well, the expert in this

would be Gordon Mitchell.

I would say it moves

in that direction,

but it’s not as extreme

because I don’t think

you can get down

to the levels of hypoxia

that they do clinically.

I know that our pals

at our Breath Collective

actually just bought a machine

because you can buy a machine

that does this.

I see.

And they bought it

and they’re going to do

their own self-testing

to see whether or not

this has any effect

on anything that they can measure.

Of course, you have to be concerned

about self-experimentation,

but I applaud their curiosity

in going after it.

Hyperbaric chambers.

I hear a lot nowadays

about hyperbaric chambers.

People are buying them

and using them.

What are your thoughts

on hyperbaric chambers

as it relates to any of the-

Hyper or hypo?

Hyperbaric chambers.

Okay, so you’re not talking

about altitude?

No.

I don’t really have much to say.

I mean, your oxygen levels

will probably go up a little bit

and that could have

a beneficial effect,

but that’s way outside

my area of comfort.

I think 2022,

I think is going to be

the year of two things

I keep hearing a lot about,

which is the deliberate use

of high salt intake

for performance,

increasing blood volume, et cetera,

and hyperbaric chambers

seem to be catching on

much in the same way

that ice baths were

and saunas seem to be right now.

But anyway, a prediction

we can return to at some point.

I want to ask you about

some of the studies

that I’ve seen out there

exploring how deliberately

restricting one’s breathing

to nasal breathing

can do things like improve memory.

There’s a couple of papers

in Journal of Neuroscience,

which is a respectable journal

in our field.

One looking at olfactory memory.

So that kind of made sense

because you can smell things

better through your nose

than your mouth,

unless you’re some sort of elk

or something where they can,

presumably they have

some sense of smell

in their mouth as well.

But humans generally smell

with their nose.

That wasn’t terribly surprising,

but there was a companion study

that showed that the hippocampus,

an area involved in encoding memories

in one form or another,

was more active, if you will,

and memory and recall

was better when people

learned information

while nasal breathing

as opposed to mouth breathing.

Does that make sense

from any mechanistic perspective?

Well, given that there are,

there’s a major pathway

going from the olfactory system

into the brain,

and you cut that,

and not one from any receptors

in the mouth,

the degree of respiratory modulation

you’re going to see

throughout the forebrain

is going to be less

with mouth breathing

than nose breathing.

So it’s certainly plausible.

I think there are a lot of experiments

that need to be done

to distinguish between

the two that is the nasal component

and the non-nasal component

of these breathing-related signals.

There’s a tendency sometimes

when you have a strong effect

to be exclusive,

and I think what’s going on here

is that there are many inputs

that can have an effect.

Now, whether they’re parceled,

that some affect this part of behavior

and some affect that part of behavior

remains to be investigated.

There’s certainly a strong olfactory component,

my interest is trying to follow

the central component

because we know that there’s

a strong central component in this.

In fact, there’s a strong central projection

to the olfactory bulb.

So regardless of whether or not

there’s any air flowing in and out of the nose,

there’s a respiratory input

into the olfactory bulb,

which combines with the respiratory

modulated signals coming

from the sensory receptors.

Interesting.

And as long as we are poking around,

forgive the pun, the nose,

what about one nostril versus the other nostril?

I know it sounds a little crazy to imagine,

but there have been theories in yoga traditions

and others that breathing through one nostril

somehow activates certain brain centers,

maybe hemispherically,

one side of the brain versus the other,

or that right nostril and left nostril breathing

might differ in terms of the levels

of alertness or calmness they produce.

I’m not aware of any mechanistic data on that,

but if there’s anything worthwhile

about right nostril versus left nostril breathing

that you’re aware of, I’d love to know.

Well, it’s certainly plausible.

I don’t know of any data demonstrating it

except the anecdotal reports.

As you know, the brain is highly lateralized

and we have speech on one side

and a dominant hand is on one side.

And so the notion that if you have this huge signal

coming from the olfactory system

and to some degree it’s lateralized,

it’s not perfectly symmetrical,

that is, one side is not going evenly to both sides,

then you can imagine that once the signal gets distributed

in a way that’s not uniform,

that the effectiveness or the response

is gonna be particular to the cortex

in which either the signal still remains

or the signal is removed from.

I see.

What are some of the other features of our brain and body,

be it blinking or eye movements

or ability to encode sounds

or any features of the way that we function

and move and perceive things

that are coordinated with breathing

in some interesting way?

Thank you for that question.

Almost everything.

So we have, for example, on the autonomic side,

we have respiratory sinus arrhythmia.

That is, during expiration, the heart slows down.

Your pupils oscillate with the respiratory cycle.

I don’t know what the functional basis for that is,

but they do oscillate with the respiratory cycle.

When we inhale, our pupils constrict presumably

because there’s an increase in heart rate

and sympathetic tone.

I would think of constriction.

I’m guessing as you relax, the pupil will get,

and you exhale, the pupil will get-

I think you’re right, but I always get the valence of that.

Well, it’s counterintuitive

because people wouldn’t think that when the pupils get,

I mean, it depends.

I mean, you can get very alert and aroused

for stress or for good reasons,

and the pupils get wider, but your visual field narrows,

and then the opposite is true.

Anyway, I guess the idea is

that the pupils are changing size,

and therefore the aperture of your visual window

is changing in coordination with breathing.

Okay.

Your fear response changes with the respiratory cycle.

Tell us more about that.

Well, it was a paper by Zolano,

which I think showed rather clearly

that if you show individuals fearful faces,

that their measured response of fearfulness

changes between inspiration and expiration.

You know, I don’t know why, but it does.

Your reaction time changes.

So you talk about blinking.

The reaction time changes

between inspiration and expiration.

If I ask you to punch something,

the time will change between inspiration and expiration.

In fact, I don’t know the degree

to which martial artists exploit that.

You know, you watch the breathing pattern,

and your opponent will actually move slower

during one cycle compared to the other.

Meaning, in which direction?

If they’re exhaling, they can punch faster?

I have to say, I don’t keep a table

of which direction things move in

because I’m out of the martial arts field now.

My vague understanding is that exhales on strikes

is the more typical way to do that.

And so as people strike, they exhale.

In many martial arts-

No, as you exhale.

But there are other components to striking

because you want to stiffen your rib cage,

you want to make a Valsalva maneuver.

So that’s both an inspiration and an expiration.

It’s at the same time.

So I don’t know enough about,

when you say during expiration,

I would assume that when you’re making a strike,

you actually sort of want to stiffen here,

which is a Valsalva-like maneuver.

And oftentimes, they’ll clinch their fist

at the last moment.

Anyway, there’s a whole set of motor things there

that we can talk to some fighters.

We know people who know fighters, so we can ask them.

Interesting, what are some other things

that are modulated by breathing?

I think anything anyone looks at

seems to have a breathing component

because it’s all over your brain.

And it’s hard to imagine it not being effective.

Now, whether it’s incidental or just background

and doesn’t really have any behavioral advantage

is possible.

In other cases, it might have a behavioral advantage.

I mean, the big eye-opening thing for me,

probably a decade ago, was digging into literature

and seeing how much of cortical activity

and subcortical activity

had a respiratory modulated component to it.

And I think a lot of my colleagues

who are studying cortex are oblivious to this.

And they find, I heard a talk the other day

with a person who had gone unnamed,

who find a lot of things correlated

with a particular movement.

And I think it all, when I looked, I said,

gee, that’s a list of things that are respiratory modulated.

And rather than it being correlated

to the movement that we’re looking at,

I think the movement they were looking at

was modulated by breathing, as was everything else.

So there wasn’t that the movement itself

was driving that correlation.

It was that they were all correlated to something else,

which is the breathing movement.

And whether or not that is a behaviorally relevant

or behaviorally something you can exploit, I don’t know.

I suspect you’re right, that breathing is,

if not the foundational driver of many,

if not all of these things,

that it’s at least one of the foundational drivers.

It’s in the background, it’s in the brain,

and oscillations play an important part in brain function.

And they vary in frequency from maybe 100 hertz

down to, well, we can get to circadian

and sort of monthly cycles.

But breathing occupies a rather unusual place in all that,

because, so let me talk about what people think

the oscillations are doing, particularly the faster ones.

They’re important in coordinating signals across neurons.

Just like in a computer, a computer steps.

So a computer knows when information is coming

from another part of a computer

that it was originated at a particular time.

And so that discrete step-by-step thing

is important in computer control.

Now, the brain is not a digital device,

it’s an analog device.

But when I have a signal coming in my ear and my eye,

which is Andrew Huberman speaking,

and I’m looking at his face, I see that as a whole,

but the signal is coming into different parts of my brain.

How do I unify that?

Well, my neurons are very sensitive to changes

in signals arriving by fractions of a millisecond.

So how do I assure that those signals coming in

represent the same signal?

Well, if I have throughout my brain an oscillation,

and the signals ride on that oscillation,

let’s say the peak of the oscillation,

I can then have a much better handle

on the relative timing and say,

those two signals came in at the same time,

they may relate to the same object,

and aha, I see you as one unified thing spouting,

you know, talking.

And so these oscillations come

in many different frequency ranges,

and are important in memory formation

and all sorts of things.

I don’t think people pay much attention to breathing

because it’s relatively slow to this,

the range when you think about milliseconds.

But we have important things

that are thought to be important in cognitive function,

which are a few cycles per second,

to 20, 30, 40, 50 cycles per second.

Breathing in humans is maybe 0.2 cycles per second,

every five seconds, although in rodents,

they’re up to four per second, which is pretty fast.

So, but breathing has one thing which is special,

that is, you can readily change it.

So the degree to which the brain

is using that slow signal for anything,

if that becomes part of its normal signal processing,

you now change it,

that signal processing has to change.

And as that signal processing changes,

acutely, there’s a change.

So, you know, you asked about breath practice,

how long do you have to do it?

Well, a single breath will change your state.

You know, you’re nervous, you take a deep breath,

and it seems to help relax, or even sigh.

Call it what you will.

Call it what you will.

It seems to work.

Now, it doesn’t have a permanent change,

but, you know, when I’m getting up to bat,

or getting up to the first tee,

or getting to give a big talk,

or coming to do a podcast, get a little bit anxious,

a deep breath or a few deep breaths

are tremendously effective in calming one down.

And so, you can get a transient disruption,

but on the other hand,

let’s take something like depression.

I think it’s, you can envision depression

as activity sort of going around in a circuit.

And because it’s continuous in the nervous system,

as signals keep repeating, they tend to get stronger.

And they can get so strong, you can’t break them.

So, you can imagine depression

being something going on and on and on,

and you can’t break it.

And so, we have trouble when we get

to certain levels of depression.

I mean, all of us get depressed at some point,

but if it’s not continuous, it’s not long lasting,

we’re able to break it.

But if it’s long lasting and very deep, we can’t break it.

So, the question is, how do we break it?

Well, there are extreme measures to break it.

We could do electroconvulsive shock.

We shock the whole brain.

That’s disrupting activity in the whole brain.

And when the circuit starts to get back together again,

it’s been disruptive.

And we know that the brain,

when signals get disrupted a little bit,

we can weaken the connections.

And weakening the connections,

if it’s that in the circuit involved in depression,

we may get some relief.

And electroconvulsive shock does work

for relieving many kinds of depression.

That’s pretty heroic.

Focal deep brain stimulation does the same thing,

but more localized or transcranial stimulation.

You’re disrupting a network,

and while it’s getting back together,

it may weaken some of the connections.

If breathing is playing some role in this circuit,

and now, instead of doing like a one-second shock,

I do 30 minutes of disruption by doing slow breathing

or other breathing practice,

those circuits begin to break down a little bit,

and I get some relief.

And if I continue to do it

before the circuit can then build back up again,

I gradually can wear that circuit down.

I sort of liken this,

I tell people it’s like walking around on a dirt path.

You build a rut.

The rut gets so deep, you can’t get out of it.

And what breathing is doing

is sort of filling in the rut bit by bit

to the point that you can climb out of that rut.

And that is because breathing,

the breathing signal is playing some role

in the way the circuit works.

And then when you disrupt it,

the circuit gets a little thrown off kilter.

And as you know, when circuits get thrown off,

the nervous system tries to adjust in some way or another.

And it turns out, at least for breathing,

for some evolutionary reason or just by happenstance,

it seems to improve our emotional function

or our cognitive function.

And we’re very fortunate that that’s the case.

It’s a terrific segue into what I want to ask you next.

And this is part of a set of questions

I want to make sure we touch on before we wrap up,

which is what do you do with all this knowledge

in terms of a breathing practice?

You mentioned that one breath can shift your brain state

and that itself can be powerful.

I think that’s absolutely true.

You’ve also talked about 30-minute breathwork practices,

which is 30 minutes of breathwork

is a pretty serious commitment, I think, but it’s doable.

Certainly a zero cost except for the time in most cases.

What do you see out there in the landscape of breathwork

that’s being done that you like?

And why do you like it?

What do you think you,

or what would you like to see more of

in terms of exploration of breathwork?

And what do you do?

Well, I’m a relatively new convert to breathwork.

Through my own investigation of it,

I became convinced that it’s real.

And I’m basically a beginner in terms of my own practice.

And I like to keep things simple.

And I think I’ve discussed this before.

I liken it to someone who’s a couch potato

who’s told they got to begin to exercise.

You don’t go out and run a marathon.

So, you know, couch potato, you say,

okay, get up and walk for five minutes, then 10 minutes.

And then, okay, now you’re walking for a longer period,

you begin to run.

And then you reach a point, you say,

well, gee, I’m interested in this sport.

And there may be particular kinds of practices

that you can use that could be helpful

in optimizing performance of that sport.

I’m not there yet.

I find I get tremendous benefit by relatively short periods

between five and maybe 20 minutes of doing box breathing.

It’s very simple to do.

I have a simple app, which helps me keep the timing.

Do you recall which app it is?

Is it the Apnea Trainer?

Is that the one?

Well, I was using Calm for a long time,

but I let my subscription relapse.

And I have another one whose name I don’t remember.

But it’s, so it’s very simple and it works for me.

Now trying this Tummo,

because I’m just curious and exploring it

because it may be acting for a different way.

And I wanna see if I respond differently.

So I don’t have a particular point of view now.

I have friends and colleagues who are into,

you know, particular styles like Wim Hof.

And I think what he’s doing is great

in getting people who are interested.

I think the notion is that I would like to see

more people exploring this.

And to some degree, as you point out, 30 minutes a day,

some of the breath patterns that some of these stars

like Wim Hof are a little intimidating to newbies.

And so I would like to see something very simple

that people, what I tell my friends is,

look, just try it five or 10 minutes.

See if you feel better, do it for a few days.

If you don’t like it, stop it, it doesn’t cost anything.

And invariably they find that it’s helpful.

I will often interrupt my day

to take five or 10 minutes.

Like if I find that I’m lagging, you know,

there’s a, I think there’s some pretty good data

that your performance after lunch declines.

And so very often what I’ll do after lunch,

which I didn’t do today, is take five or 10 minutes

and just sort of breath practice.

And lately, what does that breath practice look like?

It’s just box breathing for five or 10 minutes.

And the duration of your inhales and holds and exhales

and holds is set by the app, is that right?

Well, I do five seconds.

So five seconds inhale, five second hold,

five second exhale, five second hold.

And sometimes I’ll do doubles.

I’ll do 10 seconds.

Just because I get bored, you know,

it’s just, I feel like doing it.

And it’s very helpful.

I mean, now that’s not the only thing I do

with respect to trying to maintain my sanity and my health.

No, I can imagine that there’d be a number of things.

Although you seem, because you seem very sane

and very healthy, I in fact know that you are.

Both of those things.

Well, you suspect that I am.

I suspect, but there’s data.

A while back, we had a conversation, a casual conversation,

but you said something that really stuck in my mind,

which is that it might be that the specific pattern

of breath work that one does is not as important

as experiencing transitions between states

based on deliberate breath work or something to that extent,

which I interpreted to mean that

if I were to do box breathing with five second in,

five second hold, five second exhale,

five second hold for a couple of days,

or maybe even a couple of minutes,

and then switch to 10 seconds, or then switch to TUMO,

that there’s something powerful, perhaps,

in the transitions and realizing the relationship

between different patterns of breathing

in those transitions, much in the same way

that you can get into one of these cars

at an amusement park that just goes at a constant rate

and then stops, very different than learning

how to shift gears, you know, I used to drive a manual,

I still can, so I’m thinking about a manual transmission,

but even with an automatic transmission,

you start to get a sense of how the vehicle behaves

under different conditions,

and I thought that was a beautiful seed

for a potential breath work practice

that, at least to my awareness, nobody has really formalized

which is that you introduce some variability

within the practice that’s somewhat random

in order to be able to sense the relationship

between different speeds and depths of inhales,

exhales, and holds, and so forth,

and essentially, it’s like driving around the track,

but with obstacles at different rates,

and braking, and restarting, and things of that sort,

that’s how you learn how to drive.

What do you think about that, and if you like it enough,

can we call it the Feldman Protocol?

Oh, please.

You know, I was asked in this BBC interview once,

why didn’t I name it the Feldman Complex,

instead of the Prebutzinger Complex?

You said I already have a Feldman Complex.

Well, it sounds like a psychiatric disorder,

but I think the primary effect

is this disruptive effect, which I described,

but the particular responses may clearly vary

depending on what that disruption is.

I don’t know of any particular data

which are some well-controlled experiments

which can actually work through

the different types of breathing patterns,

or simply with a box pattern, just varying the durations.

I mean, prayama is sort of similar,

but the amount of time you spend

going around the box is different.

So I don’t really have much to say about this.

I mean, this is why we need

better controlled experiments in humans,

and I think this is where being able to study it in rodents,

where you can have a wide range of perturbations

while you’re doing more invasive studies

to really get down as to which regions are affected,

how is the signal processing disrupted,

which is still a hypothesis, but how it’s disrupted

could tell us a lot about, you know,

maybe there’s a resonant point

at which there’s an optimal effect

when you take a particular breathing practice.

And then when we talked about, you know,

the fact that different breathing practices

could be affecting the outcome through different pathways.

You know, you have the olfactory pathway,

you have a central pathway, you have a vagal pathway,

you have a descending pathway,

how different practices may change

the summation of those things,

because I think all those things are probably involved.

And we’re just beginning to scratch the surface.

And I just hope that we can get serious neuroscientists

and psychologists to do the right experiments

to get at this, because I think there’s a lot of value

to human health here.

I do too, and it’s one of the reasons my lab

has shifted to these sorts of things in humans.

I’m delighted that you’re continuing to do

the hardcore mechanistic work in mice

and probably do work in humans as well,

if you’re not already.

And there are other groups, EPOL lab at UCSF,

and a number of, I’m starting to see some papers out there

about respiration in humans a little bit,

some more brain imaging.

I can’t help but ask about a somewhat unrelated topic,

but it is important in light of this conversation

because you’re here.

And one of the things that I really enjoy

about conversations with you,

as it relates to health and neuroscience and so forth,

is that you’re one of the few colleagues I have

who openly admits to exploring supplementation.

I’m a long time supplement fan.

I think there’s power in compounds,

both prescription, non-prescription, natural, synthesized.

I don’t use these haphazardly,

but I think there’s certainly power in them.

And one of the places where you and I converge

in terms of our interest in the nervous system

and supplementation is vis-a-vis magnesium.

Now, I’ve talked endlessly on the podcast and elsewhere

about magnesium for sake of sleep

and improving transitions to sleep and so forth.

But you have a somewhat different interest in magnesium

as it relates to cognitive function

and durability of cognitive function.

Would you mind just sharing with us a little bit

about what that interest is, where it stems from?

And because it’s the Huberman Lab podcast

and we often talk about supplementation,

what you do with that information?

So I need to disclose that I am a scientific advisor

to a company called Neuroscentria,

which my graduate student, Guo-Sang Liu, is CEO.

So that said, I can give you some background.

Guo-Sang, although when he was in my lab,

worked on breathing,

had a deep interest in learning and memory.

And when he left my lab, he went to work

with a renowned learning and memory guy

at Stanford, Dick Chen.

And when he finished there,

he was hired by Susumu Tonogawa at MIT.

Who also knows a thing or two about memory.

I’m teasing.

Susumu has a Nobel for his work on immunoglobulins,

but then is a world-class memory researcher.

And more.

He’s many things.

And Guo-Sang had very curious, very bright guy.

And he was interested in how signals

between neurons get strengthened,

which is called long-term potentiation or LTP.

And one of the questions that arose

was if I have inputs to a neuron,

and I get LTP,

is the LTP bigger if the signal is bigger,

or the noise is less?

So we can imagine that when we’re listening to something,

if it’s louder, we can hear it better.

Or if there’s less noise, we can hear it better.

And he wanted to investigate this.

So he did this in tissue culture of hippocampal neurons.

And what he found was that if he

lowered the background activity in all of the neurons,

that the LTP he elicited got stronger.

And the way he did that was increasing

the level of magnesium in the bathing solution.

This gets into some esoteric electrophysiology.

But basically, there’s a background level

of noise in all neurons.

And that part of it is regulated

by the degree of magnesium in the extracellular bath.

And you mean electrical noise.

Electrical noise, I’m sorry, electrical noise.

And if you, in what’s called the physiological range,

which is between 0.8 and 1.2 millimolar,

which don’t worry about the number.

I can’t believe I remember the millimolar of the magnesium.

I’m always frightened that I get,

if I say micro or femto or something,

I go off by several orders of magnitude.

So in that physiological range,

there’s a big difference in the amount of noise

in a neuron between 0.8 and 1.2 millimolar.

So he played around with the magnesium

and he found out that when the magnesium was elevated,

there was more LTP.

All right, that’s an observation in a tissue culture.

Right, and I should just mention that more LTP

essentially translates to more neuroplasticity,

more rewiring of connections in essence.

So he tested this in mice

and basically he offered them a,

he had control mice, which got a normal diet

and one that had, one enriched with magnesium.

And the ones that lived enriched with magnesium

had higher cognitive function, lived longer,

everything you’d want in some magic pill,

those mice did that, excuse me, rats.

The problem was that you couldn’t imagine

taking this into humans because most magnesium salts

don’t passively get from the gut

into the bloodstream, into the brain.

They pass via a, what’s called a transporter.

A transporter is something in a membrane

that grabs a magnesium molecule or atom

and pulls it into the other side.

So if you imagine you have magnesium in your gut,

you have transporters that pull the magnesium

into the gut, into the bloodstream.

Well, if you had taken normal magnesium supplement

that you can buy at the pharmacy,

it doesn’t cross the gut very easily.

And if you would take enough of it

to get it in your bloodstream, you start getting diarrhea.

So it’s not a good way to go.

Well, it is a good way to go.

Couldn’t help myself.

Well said.

So he worked with this brilliant chemist, Fay Mao,

and Fay looked at a whole range of magnesium compounds

and he found that magnesium threonate

was much more effective in crossing the gut blood barrier.

Now, they didn’t realize at the time,

but threonate is a metabolite of vitamin C.

And there’s lots of threonate in your body.

So magnesium threonate would appear to be safe

and maybe part of the role,

or now they believe it’s part of the role of the threonate,

is that it supercharges the transporter

to get the magnesium in.

And remember, you need a transporter at the gut,

into the brain and into cells.

So they gave magnesium threonate to mice who had,

no, let me backtrack a bit.

They did a study in humans.

They hired a company to do a test.

It was a hands-off test.

It’s one of these companies that gets hired

by the big pharma to do their test for them.

And they got patients who were diagnosed

as mild cognitive decline.

These are people who had cognitive disorder,

which was age-inappropriate.

And the metric that they use

for determining how far off they were

is Spearman’s g-factor,

which is a generalized measure of intelligence

that most psychologists accept.

And the biological age of the subjects was,

I think, 51.

And the cognitive age was 61

based on the Spearman g-test.

Oh, I should say, the Spearman g-factor

starts at a particular level in the population at age 20

and declines about 1% a year.

So sorry to say, we’re not 20-year-olds anymore.

But when you get a number from that,

you can put on the curve

and see whether it’s about your age or not.

These people were about 10 years older

according to that metric.

And long story short, after three months,

this was a placebo-controlled double-blind study.

The people who were in the placebo arm

improved two years,

which is common for human studies

to cause a placebo effect.

The people who got the compound

improved eight years on average.

And some improved more than eight years.

They didn’t do any further diagnosis

as to what caused the mild cognitive decline.

But it was pretty, it was extraordinarily impressive.

So it moved their cognition closer to their biological age.

Biological age.

Do you recall what the doses of magnesium-3 and 8 were?

It’s in the paper,

and it’s basically what they have in the compound

which is sold commercially.

So the compound which is sold commercially

is handled by a nutraceutical wholesaler

who sells it to the retailers,

and they make whatever formulation they want.

But it’s a dosage which is,

my understanding, is rarely tolerable.

I take half a dose.

The reason I take half a dose

is that I had my magnesium, blood magnesium measured,

and it was low normal for my age.

I took half a dose, it became high normal.

And I felt comfortable staying in the normal range.

But, you know, a lot of people are taking the full dose.

And at my age, I’m not looking to get smarter.

I’m looking to decline more slowly.

And it’s hard for me to tell you

whether or not it’s effective or not.

Well, you remembered the millimolar

of the magnesium in the solution,

and on the high and low end.

So I would say it’s not a well-controlled study,

and it’s an N of one, but it seems to be working.

When I recommend it to my friends,

academics who are, by nature, skeptical, if not cynical,

and I insist that they try it,

they usually don’t report a major change

in their cognitive function,

although sometimes they do report,

well, I feel a little bit more alert,

and my physical movements are better.

But many of them report they sleep better.

And that makes sense.

I think there’s good evidence that three and eight

can accelerate the transition into sleep,

and maybe even access to deeper modes of sleep.

For many people, actually,

a small percentage of people who take three and eight,

including one of our podcast staff here,

have stomach issues with it.

They can’t tolerate it.

I would say, just anecdotally,

about 5% of people don’t tolerate three and eight well.

You stop taking it, and then they’re fine.

It causes them diarrhea or something of that sort.

But most people tolerate it well,

and most people report that it vastly improves their sleep.

And again, that’s anecdotally.

There are a few studies, and they’re more on the way.

That’s very interesting,

because until you and I had the discussion

about three and eight,

I wasn’t aware of the cognitive enhancing effects.

But the story makes sense from a mechanistic perspective.

And it brings it around

to a bigger and more important statement,

which is that I so appreciate your attention to mechanism.

I guess this stems from your early training as a physicist,

and the desire to get numbers,

and to really parse things at a fine level.

So we’ve covered a lot today.

I know there’s much more that we could cover.

I’m going to insist on a part two at some point,

but I really want to speak on behalf

of a huge number of people, and just thank you,

not just for your time and energy and attention

to detail and accuracy and clarity around this topic today,

but also what I should have said at the beginning,

which is that you really are a pioneer

in this field of studying respiration,

and the mechanisms underlying respiration

with modern tools now for many decades.

And the field of neuroscience was one

that was perfectly content to address issues

like memory and vision and sensation, perception, et cetera.

But the respiratory system was largely overlooked

for a long time,

and you’ve just been steadily clipping away

and clipping away,

much because of the events related to COVID

and a number of other things,

and this huge interest in breathwork

and brain states and wellness.

The field of respiration and interest in respiration

has just exploded.

So I really want to extend a sincere thanks.

It means a lot to me,

and I know to the audience of this podcast

that someone with your depth and rigor in this area

is both a scientist and a practitioner,

and that you would share this with us, so thank you.

Well, I want to thank you.

This is actually a great opportunity for me.

I’ve been isolated in my silo for a long time,

and it’s been a wonderful experience

to communicate to people outside the silo

who have an interest in this,

and I think that there’s a lot that remains to be done,

and I enjoy speaking to people who have interest in this.

I find the interest to be quite mind-boggling,

and it’s quite wonderful

that people are willing to listen to things

that can be quite esoteric at times,

but it gets down to deep things about who we are

and how we are going to live our lives.

So I appreciate the opportunity,

and I would be delighted to come back at any time.

Wonderful, we will absolutely do it.

Thanks again, Jack.

Bye now.

Thank you for joining me

for my conversation with Dr. Jack Feldman.

I hope you found it as entertaining

and as informative as I did.

If you’re learning from and or enjoying this podcast,

please subscribe to us on YouTube.

That’s a terrific zero-cost way to support us.

In addition, please subscribe to the podcast

on Spotify and Apple,

and on Apple, you can leave us a review

and you can leave us up to a five-star rating.

Please also check out the sponsors mentioned

at the beginning of the podcast.

That’s the best way to support this podcast.

In addition, if you’re not already following us

on Instagram and Twitter,

I teach neuroscience on Instagram and Twitter,

so some of that information covers information

covered on the podcast.

Some of that information is unique information,

and that includes science and science-based tools

that you can apply in everyday life.

And as mentioned at the beginning of today’s episode,

we are now partnered with Momentus Supplements

because they make single ingredient formulations

that are of the absolute highest quality,

and they ship international.

If you go to livemomentus.com slash Huberman,

you will find many of the supplements

that have been discussed on various episodes

of the Huberman Lab podcast,

and you will find various protocols

related to those supplements.

I also want to just mention one more time,

the program that I mentioned

at the beginning of the episode,

which is Our Breath Collective.

The Our Breath Collective has an advisory board

that includes people like Dr. Jack Feldman,

where you can learn detailed breathwork protocols.

If you’re interested in doing or teaching breathwork,

I highly recommend checking it out.

You can find it at ourbreathcollective.com slash Huberman,

and that will give you $10 off your first month.

So I want to thank you once again for joining me

for my conversation with Dr. Jack Feldman.

And last, but certainly not least,

thank you for your interest in science.

comments powered by Disqus