Welcome to the Huberman Lab Podcast,
where we discuss science
and science-based tools for everyday life.
I’m Andrew Huberman,
and I’m a professor of neurobiology and ophthalmology
at Stanford School of Medicine.
Today, my guest is Dr. Nolan Williams.
Dr. Williams is a medical doctor
and professor of psychiatry and behavioral sciences
at Stanford University School of Medicine.
His laboratory and clinic focus on depression
and other mood disorders.
They focus specifically on the use
of transcranial magnetic stimulation,
which is a brain stimulation technique
that can either activate or quiet specific brain circuits,
as well as circuits within the body,
in order to treat depression and other mood disorders.
Other laboratories and clinics use TMS.
What sets apart the work of Nolan Williams and colleagues
is that they combine TMS with other treatments.
And some of those treatments are among the more cutting edge
that you’ve probably heard about these days,
including ibogaine, psilocybin, MDMA, cannabis, DMT,
and other drugs that at this point in time
are experimental in terms of clinical trials,
but that at least the preliminary data show
hold great promise for the treatment of depression
and other mood disorders.
In the course of my discussion with Dr. Williams,
we covered things such as the history
of each of these drugs,
how they came to be and their current status
in terms of their clinical use and legality.
We also talk about their safety profiles,
both in children and in adults.
And we talk about what the future of psychedelic research
and clinical use really looks like.
For instance, we discuss how a number of laboratories
and clinics are modifying psychedelics
to remove some of their hallucinogenic properties
while maintaining some of their antidepressant
or antitrauma properties.
You’ll also learn about some fascinating research
in Dr. Williams’ laboratory focused on ketamine,
which is a drug that is increasingly being used
to treat depression.
And contrary to common belief,
the effects of ketamine in terms of relieving depression
may not actually arise from its dissociative effects.
One thing that you’ll find extraordinary about Dr. Williams
is that not only does he have vast knowledge
of the various treatments for depression,
but that he and his laboratory
are really combining these treatments
in the most potent way.
That is combining psychedelic treatments
with brain machine interface
or combining brain machine interface
with particular learning protocols,
that is neuroplasticity protocols,
which can directly change the brain in specific ways.
So today you’re going to learn a tremendous amount
about the neural circuitry underlying depression
as well as positive moods.
You’ll also learn about all the various drugs
that I described,
and you’re really going to learn about the current status
and future of the treatment of mood disorders.
Today, you’ll also learn about a number of ongoing studies
in Dr. Williams’ laboratory.
I should mention that they are recruiting subjects
for these studies.
If you go to BSL,
which stands for Brain Stimulation Laboratory,
so that’s bsl.stanford.edu,
you have the opportunity to apply
for one of these clinical trials
for the treatment of depression and other mood disorders.
I confess that the conversation with Dr. Williams
was for me one of the more stimulating
and informative conversations I’ve ever had
about psychedelics,
which is simply to say that his breadth
and depth of knowledge on that topic is incredible,
and his breadth and depth of knowledge
in terms of the underlying brain science
and how it can all be combined with clinical applications
is also extraordinary.
I’m sure that by the end of today’s episode,
you’re going to come away
with a tremendous amount of knowledge
about the clinical and non-clinical uses
of those substances,
and you’re going to understand a lot more
about how the healthy and diseased brain work.
Before we begin,
I’d like to emphasize that this podcast
is separate from my teaching and research roles at Stanford.
It is, however, part of my desire and effort
to bring zero cost to consumer information
about science and science-related tools
to the general public.
In keeping with that theme,
I’d like to thank the sponsors of today’s podcast.
Our first sponsor is Element.
Element is an electrolyte drink
with everything you need and nothing you don’t.
That means plenty of salt, magnesium, and potassium,
the so-called electrolytes, and no sugar.
Now, salt, magnesium, and potassium
are critical to the function of all the cells in your body,
in particular, to the function of your nerve cells,
also called neurons.
In fact, in order for your neurons to function properly,
all three electrolytes need to be present
in the proper ratios,
and we now know that even slight reductions
in electrolyte concentrations or dehydration of the body
can lead to deficits in cognitive and physical performance.
Element contains a science-backed electrolyte ratio
of 1,000 milligrams, that’s one gram, of sodium,
200 milligrams of potassium,
and 60 milligrams of magnesium.
I typically drink Element first thing in the morning
when I wake up in order to hydrate my body
and make sure I have enough electrolytes,
and while I do any kind of physical training
and after physical training as well,
especially if I’ve been sweating a lot.
If you’d like to try Element,
you can go to drinkelement, that’s lmnt.com slash Huberman,
to claim a free Element sample pack with your purchase.
Again, that’s drinkelement, lmnt.com slash Huberman.
Today’s episode is also brought to us by Thesis.
Thesis makes what are called nootropics,
which means smart drugs.
Now, to be honest, I am not a fan of the term nootropics.
I don’t believe in smart drugs in the sense
that I don’t believe that there’s any one substance
or collection of substances that can make us smarter.
I do believe, based on science, however,
that there are particular neural circuits
and brain functions that allow us to be more focused,
more alert, access creativity, be more motivated, et cetera.
That’s just the way that the brain works,
different neural circuits for different brain states.
Thesis understands this, and as far as I know,
they’re the first nootropics company
to create targeted nootropics for specific outcomes.
I’ve been using Thesis for more than six months now,
and I can confidently say that their nootropics
have been a total game changer.
My go-to formula is the Clarity formula,
or sometimes I’ll use their Energy formula before training,
to get your own personalized nootropic starter kit,
go online to takethesis.com slash Huberman,
take a three-minute quiz,
and Thesis will send you four different formulas
to try in your first month.
That’s takethesis.com slash Huberman,
and use the code Huberman at checkout
for 10% off your first order.
I’m pleased to announce that the Huberman Lab Podcast
is now partnered with Momentus Supplements.
We partnered with Momentus for several important reasons.
First of all, they ship internationally,
because we know that many of you are located
outside of the United States.
Second of all, and perhaps most important,
the quality of their supplements is second to none,
both in terms of purity and precision
of the amounts of the ingredients.
Third, we’ve really emphasized supplements
that are single ingredient supplements,
and that are supplied in dosages
that allow you to build a supplementation protocol
that’s optimized for cost,
that’s optimized for effectiveness,
and that you can add things and remove things
from your protocol in a way
that’s really systematic and scientific.
If you’d like to see the supplements
that we partnered with Momentus on,
you can go to livemomentus.com slash Huberman.
There you’ll see those supplements,
and just keep in mind that we are constantly expanding
the library of supplements available through Momentus
on a regular basis.
Again, that’s livemomentus.com slash Huberman.
And now for my discussion with Dr. Nolan Williams.
Thanks for joining today.
I’m really excited to have this conversation.
It’s been a long time coming,
and I have a lot of questions about different compounds,
psychedelics in particular.
But before we get into that discussion,
I want to ask you about depression, broadly speaking,
intractable depression, how common depression is or isn’t.
I heard you say in a wonderful talk that you gave
that depression is perhaps
the most debilitating condition worldwide.
And yet in contrast to other medical conditions like cancer,
we actually have a fairly limited number of tools
to approach depression.
And yet the number of tools and the potency
of those tools is growing.
So if you could educate us on depression,
I would really appreciate it.
Yeah, absolutely.
So depression is a condition that
it has a lot of manifestations.
So you can have kind of a depression
that’s primarily loss of interest.
You can have folks who feel very anxious
and they’re kind of overactive.
You can have people who don’t have any anxiety at all,
and they’re very underactive,
and they have low motivation to do anything.
So you have this huge range of symptoms
that are in that umbrella of depression.
And some of our work is to actually work
with folks like Conor Liston and Cornell
and try to actually get biotypes based off of neuroimaging
to see if we can kind of parse out
the different depression kind of presentations
and see that clinically and also see that in the brain.
Depression is the most disabling condition worldwide.
What’s interesting about depression
is it’s both a risk factor for other illnesses
and it makes other medical
and psychiatric illnesses worse, right?
So recently the American Heart Association
added depression as the fourth major risk factor
for coronary artery disease, right?
So alongside the risk factors that we know,
hypertension, high blood pressure, hyperlipidemia,
high cholesterol, and diabetes, high blood sugar.
Those three have been on the list for a long time
and depression ended up being added to the list
as the fourth one.
And really interesting, right?
So in addition to taking medications
to address those other three risk factors,
we really have to be thinking about
how do you treat folks with depression
to reduce their risk of having a heart attack in the future.
And some of that’s being worked on now,
but we don’t have a complete solution
to thinking about that at this time.
And then the other thing that’s interesting
is once you have a heart attack in the individuals
that end up having a heart attack,
the risk of having depression after the heart attack
is higher than the normal population, right?
And so a lot of what we’re doing in the lab actually
is measuring kind of brain-heart connections
and we can actually with transcranial magnetic stimulation,
a form of brain stimulation,
we can actually decelerate the heart rate
and capture that heart rate deceleration
over the mood regulatory regions.
And so actually a direct probe of that connection.
So it’s interesting.
And so, as you said a second ago,
it’s a very disabling condition, moderate depressions,
about as disabling as having a heart attack,
acutely having a heart attack.
Severe depressions as disabling
as having cancer without treatment
and dying from a cancer without treatment.
And so it’s kind of underappreciated
just how disabling depression is in that way.
And I think important as stigma is consistently
kind of being reduced over the years for mental illness,
for mental illnesses,
then the idea that we can start really putting more funding
and putting more focus at the federal level,
private foundation level, whatever it is
at a given university to thinking about
developing treatments.
We’ve been very interested in a very particular
clinical set of problems around the most severe
and the most high acuity settings
that folks with depression end up being in.
And that’s in emergency settings
where they go into inpatient units.
And in the rest of medicine,
if it’s talking about heart attacks,
if I start having chest pain right now
and you bring me to a primary care doctor’s office,
they’re gonna have a certain number of tests
and treatments, right?
But very limited because it’s an outpatient facility.
If you bring me to the emergency room after that,
there are more tests and more treatments.
If you put me in the ICU or in the cath lab
where they do invasive procedures to the heart,
there are more tests and more treatments.
In psychiatry, as we elevate the acuity of an individual,
you go from being just depressed to being depressed
and now thinking about ending your life,
the number of treatments actually go down on average.
I mean, in some scenarios they go up,
but on average they go down and there are no tests, right?
And so we’ve been very focused on that particular problem.
Somebody that maybe was doing fairly okay
with a pretty moderate depression
and their depression gets worse
and then they end up in an emergency setting
and the field really hasn’t developed a way
of consistently being able to treat that problem
and folks end up getting the same standard
oral antidepressants that they’ve been getting
from an outpatient and I came to this
because I’m dual trained as a neurologist and psychiatrist,
went back and forth between neurology and psychiatry,
saw that in neurology we have all of these ways
of treating acute brain-based problems
and really wanted to emulate that in psychiatry
and find ways to develop and engineer
new brain-based solutions.
There’s a lot to unpack there.
One thing that you said I’d like to focus on a bit more
because I think we hear that the brain
and the heart are connected, but you described,
I believe a direct relationship between areas
of the brain associated with emotion and heart rate.
And that makes perfect logical sense to me,
but I think at the same time,
many people out there probably think of the relationship
between the heart and the mind as kind of woo
or kind of a soft biology,
but here you’re talking about an actual physical connection
between what area of the brain is it?
The first place where the stimulation goes
is called the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex.
It’s kind of the sense of control,
kind of governor of the brain.
And then what we know is that when you use a magnet,
use kind of what we call Faraday’s law,
this idea of using a magnetic pulse
to induce an electrical current
in electrically conducting substances.
So in this case, brain tissue,
but not skull or scalp or any of that or hair.
You avoid all that, just the brain tissue.
Then you have a direct depolarization of cortical neurons,
you know, the surface of the brain’s neurons
in this dorsolateral prefrontal.
And if you do that in the actual scanner, which we can do,
you can see that that distributes down
into the intercingulate and the insula and the amygdala.
And ultimately the tract goes
into something called the nucleus tractus solitarius
and ultimately into the vagus nerve and to the heart.
So the heart very consistently seems to be the end organ
of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex.
If you measure heart rate in standard ways
that cardiologists measure heart rate
and you stimulate over this left dorsolateral,
you get a deceleration of the heart rate
and it’s very time locked to the stimulation.
So it’s a two second train of stimulation.
At one second you see the deceleration,
it goes down about 10 beats per minute
and then it’ll drift back up and there’s a break
for eight seconds on the stimulation,
it drifts back up and the stimulation goes back in
and then the heart rate goes back down.
So you see the heart rate just do this,
10 beats per minute every train.
And so we know if you do that over visual cortex,
you don’t get that, or motor cortex,
you don’t get any of those findings.
It’s really specific to this kind of control region
of the brain.
So yeah, it seems to, it’s our work,
other folks work, Martin Arons in Europe,
the Netherlands work showing the same connections.
I think it’s been replicated like four or five times.
So you mentioned left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex.
Anytime I hear about lateralization of function,
I get particularly curious because obviously
we have two mirror symmetric sides of the brain.
There are rare exceptions to this,
like the pineal and things of that sort
that are only, there’s only one pineal.
What is special about the left dorsolateral
prefrontal cortex?
Does this have anything to do with handedness,
right hand or left hand?
Because we know right hand and left handedness
has a lot to do with lateralization of function
for language, a topic for another time.
But why do you think that left dorsolateral
prefrontal cortex would be connected to the heart
in this way?
Yeah, yeah, I think, so left dorsolateral
is thought to be the side that when you excite it,
when you kind of do excitatory stimulation,
potentiating sort of stimulation
that you can reduce depressive symptoms.
And a guy by the name of Mike Fox at Harvard
has demonstrated that if you have strokes in the brain
that cause depression and you put them
on the human connectome, 100,000 patient map,
and you ask the question
what they’re all functionally connected to,
left dorsolateral.
If you take lesions that cause mania in individuals
and you put those all in the human connectome map
and ask what they’re all,
the one common area they’re all connected to,
it’s the right dorsolateral.
And so there seems to be a hemispheric,
balancing of mood between these two brain regions.
And we know this from an experimental standpoint too,
because you can take individuals with depression
and you can excite the left or you can inhibit the right
and they’re both antidepressant.
You can excite the right
and that’s anti-manic in some studies.
And so this idea that there is this hemispheric balancing
of mood is quite interesting, right?
It’s incredibly interesting.
And just so people know,
if you’re curious what the connectome is,
connectome is a term that was built out
of this notion of genomes
being large collections of sequencing and mapping of genes.
They’re proteomes of proteins,
of connectomes as so-called connectomics
of connections between neurons.
So the human connectome project is ongoing.
And I find that incredible
that within the connectome project
they can identify these regularities
of right versus left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex,
especially since I’ve looked at a fair number
of brains from humans,
certainly not as many as you have.
And if you look at the architecture,
the layers, the cell types,
and even the neurochemicals of which cells are expressing,
say dopamine or serotonin,
or receiving input from areas
that make dopamine or serotonin,
they don’t look that different on the right and left side.
And yet here we’re talking about
a kind of an accelerator and a brake, if you will,
on depression and mania,
using what, at least by my eye
and I think other people’s eye,
look to be basically the same set of bits,
the same parts list, more or less.
So what gives these properties
to the right and left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex?
Is it the inputs they receive?
Is this something that we learn during development?
Or do you think that we come into the world
with these hemispheric biases?
Yeah, it’s a great question.
And it hasn’t been worked out,
which your original question was,
in a left-handed individual,
which as you know, 25% of those folks
end up having a right brain dominance,
or 1% of right-handed people have a right brain dominance
if it’s flipped, right?
And that, unfortunately,
that study still hasn’t been done at the level
because that would be probably pretty helpful
for teasing some of this out.
But it’s still being sorted out, right?
We know enough to know this phenomenon exists
because we can use TMS as a probe
and do this sort of, these sorts of manipulations.
But to my knowledge, there hasn’t been anybody
that’s gotten so interested in it
that they’ve been able to get a mechanism of why that is.
But it’s kind of empirically true
in the sense that you can push and pull on those systems,
or in the case of strokes that folks have,
and then you kind of get their brains
and their brain images
and look at where the strokes landed,
those kind of causal bits of information
point to this asymmetry.
Interesting.
Well, in that case, going with what we do know,
that stimulation of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex
slows the heart rate down transiently,
but it slows it down,
and seems to alleviate at least some symptoms of depression
leads me to the question of why would that be the case?
Is it, does it tell us anything fundamental about depression
that anxiety is inherent to depression?
I think a faster heart rate is, you know,
part and parcel with anxiety.
In my laboratory, we’ve studied fear a bit
in animals and in humans,
and we often observe brachycardia,
where somebody or an animal is afraid of something,
and rather than the heart rate speeding up,
it actually slows down,
something that most people don’t think about or recognize.
But given that stimulation
of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex slows the heart rate down
and can alleviate depressive symptoms,
and that there are other ways to slow the heart down,
I have two questions.
What do you think this tells us
about the basic architecture of depression
and its physiology at the level of the heart?
And does the circuit run in the opposite direction too?
If one were to have or find other ways
to slow the heart rate down, say with a beta blocker,
does that help alleviate depression?
Yeah, no, that’s a great question.
So I’ll answer the second question first.
So we know that in the ongoing trials of this,
if you stimulate in the vagus nerve
in an implanted vagus nerve stimulator,
you can actually, you know,
have the afferent parts of the vagus project
ultimately up to the DLPFC through the cingulate
through these anterior insulas,
so obviously the same tract, right?
And you can stimulate there and alleviate depression,
which seems very unusual, right?
You’re stimulating a cranial nerve down in the neck,
but if you can get up into the brain,
you actually can improve depressive symptoms.
And so, you know, more evidence
that this is kind of a whole tract and system.
And if you stimulate in part of that system,
it appears that you can improve mood.
And what if I were somebody
who did not have a stimulating electrode in my vagus nerve,
and I was dealing with minor depression,
and I decided I wanted to take some other approach
to slow my heart rate down via the vagus,
for instance, exhale-emphasized breathing,
or deliberately slow cadence breathing,
things of that sort.
Is there any evidence that behavioral interventions
of those kinds can alleviate depression
or some symptoms of depression?
And is there any evidence that it does indeed feed back
to the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex
to achieve some of that alleviation?
Absolutely, yeah.
So there’s a number of studies implicating the dorsolateral
in, say, meditation, mindfulness, that sort of thing.
And they’re small studies,
but pretty well-designed studies
suggesting that behavioral interventions
in mild depression actually work quite well.
There seems to be a volitional threshold for depression
where at some point you start losing,
you go from being completely in total volition
to having kind of semi-volition.
You have thoughts that you really have a hard time
controlling and that sort of thing.
And when you go through that threshold,
at some point it gets harder and harder
for those sorts of things to kind of kick in and work.
And the extreme form of that is catatonia,
where people in a very severe form of depression
get kind of stuck motorically,
and they obviously can’t, they have no control,
and so, or very limited control.
And so I think there’s a threshold
in which these sorts of interventions will work.
Exercise seems to really be a good treatment
for mild depression, and it may work
through the mechanism you’re describing, right?
As we all know, athletes hold a lower resting heart rate
than folks that aren’t.
If you were an athlete, you had a lower resting heart rate,
you stopped exercising, and a couple years later
your resting heart rate, in many cases, goes up, right?
And so maybe that’s part of the process.
I’m not aware of any studies specifically
looking at dorsolateral prefrontal physiology
pre-post-exercise, but it would be a great study.
I think that would be really helpful to understanding this,
especially if you had a correlation of changes
and kind of lowering of, say, heart rate
with mood improvements.
There’s been a lot of work with heart rate variability
and depression, and studies are, kind of point towards it.
It’s not, not every study is positive for this,
but quite a few studies say, basically,
that lower heart rate variability is associated
with moderate to severe depression,
and that may be part of that mechanism
of that heart-brain risk.
So I’m both intrigued and a little bit perplexed
by this relationship between heart rate and depression.
On the face of it, I would think of depression as depressed.
So lower heart rate might make somebody more depressed.
You even mentioned catatonia, or somebody that just
doesn’t seem motivated or excited to do anything.
I think of mania as elevated heart rate and being excited.
On the other hand, I realize that anxiety,
which brings about ideas as elevated heart rate,
is also built into depression,
which brings me back to what you said earlier,
which is that when we say depression,
are we really talking about four or five different
disorders, for lack of a better word?
And for what percentage of people that have depression,
does some approach to reducing heart rate work?
Whether or not it’s stimulation
of the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex
by way of transcranial magnetic stimulation,
or by taking a beta blocker, or by stimulating the vagus.
Can we throw out a number, a rough number?
Does that help?
30%, 50%, how long-lasting is that relief?
Yeah, and to be clear, the deceleration of the heart rate
is in the moment when the stimulation is happening,
but it’s not something that’s necessarily
maintained chronically.
It’s more of an indicator that you’re in the right network,
more than it appears to be itself central to the mechanism.
The heart rate variability piece may be,
and there’s some studies that link the two,
but the actual deceleration seems to be
much more of a marker that you’re in the right system.
But it very well could be that the heart rate system
and the mood system just sit next to each other,
and the stimulation hits both.
If you look at how much of the variance in the mood
is explained by the heart rate deceleration,
it’s not a huge amount, right?
So it only explains a small percentage.
And so it’s unlikely that simply reducing the heart rate,
and in fact, for many years, propranolol
and these sorts of drugs actually were implicating
causing depression, and so that’s been kind of debunked,
but it’s unlikely that simply decelerating the heart rate
is gonna improve depression.
But what it does tell you is that if you’re in that area
that is the mood regulatory area,
there’s some parasympathetic cortical kind of process
that’s going on that gets in and causes this to happen.
And it’s independent of mood.
You can take a normal, healthy individual,
and you can do this,
and they’re gonna decelerate their heart rate.
I’m so glad you mentioned
the parasympathetic nervous system,
which, of course, most people think of
as the rest and digest or the kind of calming side
of the autonomic nervous system.
As I’m hearing you say all of this,
and in particular, what you just told me,
which is that it’s not as if having a lower heart rate
protects you against depression,
or a higher heart rate is associated with depression,
although at the extremes, that might be true,
but rather it’s something about the regulatory network,
the ability to control your own nervous system
to some extent.
And when I think about the autonomic nervous system,
I like to think about it as a seesaw
of alertness and calmness,
and when you’re asleep, it’s a lot of calmness,
and when you’re panicking, it’s a lot of alertness.
But that, and I don’t think this has ever been defined,
and when I teach the medical students
at Stanford Neuroanatomy,
my wish is that someday I’ll be able to explain
what the hinge in that process would be, right?
Not the ends of the seesaw.
We know what the sympathetic nervous system is
and what it’s, to wake us up and make us panic
or make us feel nicely alert and calm.
We know what puts someone into sleep or a coma
or makes them feel relaxed.
But what shifts from one side of the seesaw to the other
and the tightness of that hinge
seems to be what you’re describing,
that depression is sort of a lack of control
over inner state so that when I’m stressed,
I can’t get myself out of it.
But when I’m feeling completely collapsed with exhaustion,
I can’t get out of bed and get motivated
to do the very things that would help me
get out of depression, like a workout or social connection
or eat a quality meal, these kinds of things.
So this is perhaps the first time
that I’ve ever heard about a potential circuit
for the hinge as I’m referring to it.
Does that make any sense at all?
Yeah, absolutely, absolutely.
Okay, I just want to make sure
that I’m framing this correctly in my mind.
Yeah, absolutely, and in some studies,
if you do the same identical stimulation
on the right dorsolateral, you can get an acceleration.
Just kind of further confirming
this idea of lateralization, right?
That even, it appears that even the prefrontal cortex,
you know, cortical areas seem to be lateralized
in this way, and I, you know, it’s less,
the right finding is more variable
depending upon the study.
The left’s very consistent in this way, so.
So we’ve talked about transcranial magnetic stimulation
for getting into these networks.
And I also just want to take a brief tangent and say,
because I’ve heard you say this before,
I think it’s so vital what you’re saying
that it’s really not about stimulation of areas,
or any specific brain area or vagus nerve
being important per se.
It’s really about a network, a connection,
a series of connections.
I think that’s really important for people to understand,
and is kind of a new emerging theme, really.
The other thing that, to me,
seems extremely important for us to consider
is what are these lateral prefrontal cortices doing?
Are they involved, for instance, in sensation,
sensing the heart rate?
Are they involved in thinking and planning?
And this gets down to a very simple question
that I know a lot of people have,
which is, can we talk ourselves out of depression
if it’s mild?
Can we talk ourselves into a manic state,
or an excited state, a positively excited state
that doesn’t qualify as mania?
You know, other areas of the brain,
I think is responsible for perception,
or for motor control.
But here we are in this mysterious frontal cortex area,
which people say executive function, planning, et cetera.
Are we talking about thoughts?
Are we talking about structured thoughts?
Are we talking about dreamlike thoughts?
What in the world is going on in the prefrontal cortex?
And here I’ve spent my career in neuroscience,
and I still can’t really understand what it’s doing,
and maybe it’s doing 50 things.
Yeah, no, it’s a great question.
So one of the studies that we’ve been working on,
in addition to the depression work,
is actually trying to change trait hypnotizability.
So David Spiegel and I have been working on this,
and he’s found, and published this 10 years ago,
that a different part of the left dorsolateral
is functionally connected
with the dorsal anterior cingulate,
with a lot of functional connectivity in high hypnotizables,
and not much in low hypnotizables.
And that’s a different, kind of a different sub-region
within this bigger brain region
we call left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex,
than the part that seems to be important
for regulating mood.
And so the left dorsolateral seems to have connections
that are location-specific
within the overall kind of named brain region
that connect to various parts of the cingulate
and seem to regulate it, right?
And so if you knock out
the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex,
and you have people do the Stroop task, for instance,
which is a task where you have, it’s a simple task,
probably know this,
you have people name the color of words.
And so if I look at one of the cards that they’ll show you,
it’ll have the word red in red, and that’s very easy,
and that’s called a congruent.
And then the incongruent is red in the color blue,
and you have to name, you have to say the word,
you don’t name the color.
So you have to suppress a response.
Yeah, yeah, exactly.
And so, I’m sorry, you name the color
and you see the word written in a different way.
And so basically, if you stimulate in a way
that inhibits the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex
or either one, you can actually knock out
the ability to do that well,
and it’ll take longer for people in the incongruent cards
to be able to name it.
And so they have a kind of a time delay
that’s greater than they had before they got stimulated.
So that’s a part of the prefrontal cortex
that’s different than the part of the prefrontal cortex
that’s involved in mood regulation.
The nice thing about TMS is that you can go through
and you can find these areas that are functionally defined
through brain imaging, and you can perturb them
and answer the question you’re talking about.
How do I understand this part of the prefrontal cortex
and its function, this part?
And so we were able to stimulate in an inhibitory way
within the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex
that’s involved with this sort of cognitive control area,
and we were able to knock that area out
and increase trait hypnotizability.
So people had greater hypnotizability
after they got active stimulation
versus when they got sham.
And so it suggests that that brain circuit
is involved in the process
of what therapeutic hypnosis ends up being,
but it’s a very different region
within the left dorsolateral than say we do
when we do these very intensive stimulation approaches
to treat severe depression,
and we’re able to get people out of depression
with the part of the dorsolateral
that seems to be lower in the kind of more lateral
and inferior on the DLPFC
and connected with the subgenual anterior cingulate,
so the part of the anterior cingulate
that processes emotion.
I’d like to take a quick break
and acknowledge one of our sponsors, Athletic Greens.
Athletic Greens, now called AG1,
is a vitamin mineral probiotic drink
that covers all of your foundational nutritional needs.
I’ve been taking Athletic Greens since 2012,
so I’m delighted that they’re sponsoring the podcast.
The reason I started taking Athletic Greens
and the reason I still take Athletic Greens
once or usually twice a day
is that it gets me the probiotics that I need for gut health.
Our gut is very important.
It’s populated by gut microbiota
that communicate with the brain, the immune system,
and basically all the biological systems of our body
to strongly impact our immediate and long-term health.
And those probiotics in Athletic Greens
are optimal and vital for microbiotic health.
In addition, Athletic Greens contains a number of adaptogens,
vitamins, and minerals that make sure
that all of my foundational nutritional needs are met,
and it tastes great.
If you’d like to try Athletic Greens,
you can go to athleticgreens.com slash Huberman,
and they’ll give you five free travel packs
that make it really easy to mix up Athletic Greens
while you’re on the road, in the car, on the plane, et cetera,
and they’ll give you a year’s supply of vitamin D3K2.
Again, that’s athleticgreens.com slash Huberman
to get the five free travel packs
and the year’s supply of vitamin D3K2.
Based on what you told us about the Stroop Task
and the role of the prefrontal cortex in the Stroop Task,
to me, the Stroop Task is a rule-switching game.
You’re saying in one moment,
the rule is you read whatever the word says,
and then you switch, and then you say,
the rule now is you tell me what color
the word is written in,
and you suppress whatever it is the word says.
Okay?
Okay, a rule, in some sense, like that,
is a transiently adopted belief system.
So I could imagine that in depression,
which has all sorts of backstory to it,
that of course the psychiatrist or psychologist
or friend can pull on that thread.
Like for instance, somebody might believe that they are bad,
or that they don’t deserve love.
I’m trying to bring this into the typical language
that people talk about, or that they will never succeed,
or that even if they keep succeeding,
it’s just going to get harder and harder,
and it will never feel good.
These are sort of rules like the Stroop Task.
At some level, there are rules that are more pervasive
over time, unfortunately.
But I could imagine that if the PFC
is also contains some sort of maps or algorithms
related to rules of emotionality or self-representation,
or things that we’ve heard,
I think there must be data out there
that’s saying that whatever we heard in middle school,
when someone made fun of us, we can remember that.
Because I can remember things that people said
about a jacket I wore one day
or something in the fourth grade, like crazy.
I didn’t even like the jacket.
Now I think it was kind of cool.
But anyway, the point being that we have an intense memory
for these things that set up a sort of rule or a question,
like maybe I don’t really know how to dress, for instance.
Maybe that’s why I always wear the same black shirt.
But in all seriousness,
it seems like the dorsal out prefrontal cortex
is in this amazing position to access rules,
which are beliefs and beliefs are rules.
And then for moments or longer to switch those rules.
And so for somebody who’s depressed
to just simply look themselves in the mirror and say,
you are great, you are fantastic.
It feels like a lie if you feel like garbage to say that.
It doesn’t fit with the rule.
It’s like saying that card is not red.
That card is green when your eyes tell you that it’s red.
And it seems like there’s something
about prefrontal cortex that in principle
gives flexibility to rules
based on what we know about this group task.
So given its connectivity,
can we assume that the talk therapy that occurs
in the psychiatrist’s office or with a friend
or through journaling out something,
because we do know that reporting things about trauma
or difficult circumstances or the rules that we contain
and tend to hide inside of us
about how we feel miserable about ourselves
or anything really,
that in re-scripting that,
that somehow it allows us to do a sort of stoop task
on our beliefs, is that a tremendous leap?
I’m just really trying to frame this in the context
of what I and most people think of as depression.
Because the network components are vitally important,
but I guess what I’m trying to figure out is like,
what are the algorithms that govern prefrontal cortex?
Yeah, absolutely.
So in a kind of standard cognitive behavioral
therapy session, right?
What the therapist is trying to do
is identify those beliefs
and kind of determine how fixed they are,
if they’re flexible, as you’re saying,
and then help folks to find another explanation for them
and to kind of reintegrate that potential other explanation
into their memory system, right?
Where I think TMS is really interesting, actually,
we’ve had a lot of patients who’ve told me,
like, my therapist told me that I wasn’t trying hard enough
in therapy, and I really am trying hard,
but these are moderate, pretty severe depressed patients.
And as soon as we get them well with the TMS approaches,
you know, kind of rapid five-day approach,
and the next week we come in and see them,
and they’ll say, you know what I did all weekend
is I looked at my therapy books
and now I can understand it.
And so, you know, I actually see TMS
as a way of having kind of exogenous
sorts of cognitive functions
that in milder forms of depression
we can pull off with psychotherapy.
You know, this idea of being able to kind of turn
that prefrontal cortex on
and have it govern these deeper regions.
In depression, the deeper regions
govern the prefrontal cortex.
They precede the prefrontal cortex timing-wise.
We’ve got some data in review now
where we’re seeing that in depressed individuals
that are responsive to our rapid TMS approach,
we call Stanford Accelerated
Intelligent Neuromodulation Therapy,
or S&T, or SAINT,
if you look at the brain before people get this,
they will have a temporal delay
where the cingulate is in front of the DLPFC.
And in people that are normal, healthy controls,
no depression, the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex
is temporally in front of the anterior cingulate.
With effective treatment,
we can flip the timing of things
so the dorsolateral is in front of the anterior cingulate
just like in a normal person.
So you’re not talking about
obviously physically moving these structures,
you’re talking about in time, their activation.
So in one case, it’s like the coach
telling the player what to do,
and in the other case, it’s like the player
telling the coach what to do,
and you restore order to the game.
You restore order to the game.
And what it looks like is depression, to your point,
is a bunch of kind of spontaneous content
that’s semi-volitional,
that’s being kind of generated out of this
conflict detection system, the cingulate,
that seems to sense conflict
and kind of feed that information,
gets overactive in depression.
And then in depression,
it looks like the left dorsolateral
does not sufficiently clamp down on it.
And what therapy appears to do is to kind of restore that.
What we see with TMS over that region
is that we just exogenously do the same sort of thing.
We restore the governance of the left dorsolateral
over the cingulate area,
and that is correlated with treatment improvement.
So the degree in which you can re-time,
re-regulate in time,
the left dorsolateral over the cingulate,
the more of an antidepressant effect you have.
Can we therefore say in crude terms
that the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex
really is the governor of how we interpret
physiological signals and spontaneous thoughts?
It places a lens
that the rest of the brain sees things through.
And you can do these experiments
where you can put a normal, healthy control person
in the scanner and you can make them feel
like they have a loss of control,
and then you can see that region come offline.
So you can experimentally manipulate the system.
And so kind of buffing it up,
I mean, it’s like almost,
TMS is almost like exercise for the brain, right?
You’re kind of exercising this region over and over again
with a physiologically relevant signal
and kind of turning that system on.
And what’s interesting,
I think really interesting for this show is to,
you know, we had a couple of folks,
you know, probably five or six folks
that have actually told me this,
where if they remit early enough in the week,
we have this very dense stimulation approach
where we can stimulate people really rapidly
over a five-day block.
We don’t discriminate when they get better
to when they stop.
So if they get better on day one,
we still give them the other four days
because it’s in the protocol to do that.
We’re getting to a point where we can tell
how long it’s going to take,
but we’re not there yet.
And so, you know, every time somebody gets better
at day one or two,
at the beginning when we first started doing this,
we’d say, you know, we’re not sure,
you know, we think this is safe to keep going,
but you know, what do you want to do?
And everybody was like, no, I want to keep going.
And so, you know, by Wednesday,
they’re like totally zero down on the depression scales,
you know, even better than most people walking around,
like really no anxiety, no depression or anything.
By Thursday, the first guy that told me this,
he came in and he said, you know,
I was driving back to my hotel
and I decided to go to the beach
and I just sat there and I was totally present
in the present moment for an hour.
And he’s like, I read about this in my mindfulness books,
but I experienced it last night
and I’ve never experienced anything like this before.
And I was like, hmm, that’s interesting,
but kind of wasn’t sure.
And then I didn’t tell any, you know,
obviously any more patients about that.
And then about five over the last couple of years
and they get, they were met early in the week.
By the end of the week, they’re like going to the beach
and they’re like totally having what people describe
as a pretty mindful present moment sort of experience,
which is really interesting, you know, what that is.
I mean, I don’t have full on scientific data to tell you,
but it’s just, it’s an interesting anecdote, right?
That folks, when you push them through this point
of feeling kind of clinically well,
that some people end up reporting
this additional set of features, so.
Yeah, you mentioned the cingulate
and the anterior cingulate in particular,
because now I feel like for the first time in my career,
I have some sense of what prefrontal cortex
might actually be doing besides providing a bumper
for the rest of the brain.
The cingulate, it seems, is a more primitive structure
in the sense that it’s under the,
ideally it’s under the regulation of this top-down control
from prefrontal cortex, but what’s mapped in the cingulate?
And for the non-neuroscientists out there,
when I say mapped, if we were to put someone in a scanner
and focus in on cingulate or put an electrode in there,
what makes the neurons in there fire?
What sorts of things in the body and in the mind
and out in the world light up,
for lack of a better phrase, the cingulate?
What does the cingulate like?
Yeah, yeah, so that strip task,
those incongruent word color associations,
the dorsal part of that,
for obsessive-compulsive disorder patients,
certain kind of triggers you’ll see.
Some of the neuroimaging studies
will point to anterior cingulate.
In the kind of very crude psychosurgery world 50 years ago,
the anterior cingulotomy was a way
of treating obsessive-compulsive disorder, right?
Because that area seems to be overactive
in people who are experiencing
obsessive-compulsive disorder.
You can kind of walk, the cingulate wraps around
this white matter track, like bundles,
it wraps around that.
And so there’s a part that’s above that,
around that, and below that.
And depending upon how much of the conflict task
has an emotional component,
the more ventral and subgenual that activation is.
So the dorsal part of the anterior cingulate
seems to be kind of more of a pure cognitive,
maybe obsessive-compulsive disorder sort of area,
whereas when you start getting into mood sorts of triggers,
like facial expression conflicts,
where you’re supposed to,
you know, there’s an emotional strip task
where you show the word happy,
and then you have a face of a person that looks mad,
then that’s another way of having
the same sort of stroop conflict.
That seems to be more perigenual, subgenual areas, right?
So you can kind of, you can trigger the cingulate
based off the level of emotional valence
from none down to a lot.
And that seems to be how it’s distributed.
There are heart rate kind of components to it,
autonomic components in there too.
There’s something called aconetic mutism.
You know, I’m a board-certified neuropsychiatrist,
behavioral neurologist,
and I’ve seen a lot of these,
what we call zebra cases in neurology,
where people have these unusual neurological presentations,
and one of them is aconetic mutism.
So if you have a glioma
sitting in the inner hemispheric fissure
and kind of having pressure on the cingulate,
people can get into an almost catatonic-looking state
where they kind of get stuck and they don’t speak.
And so that tells you something about how the cingulate works
as well, right?
It’s like, if it’s not functioning,
then people have a hard time kind of connecting with reality.
It seems to need to be constantly online
to be able to interact with the exterior world.
Is it involved in some of the dissociative states
that sometimes people who are very stressed
or depressed experience?
You said catatonia being an extreme one,
but I know someone, for instance,
that when they get really stressed
and it can even be if someone yells at them
or someone’s angry, even if someone’s angry with them
or they perceive someone’s angry with them,
there’s a developmental backstory
to why they likely feel this way.
They sort of just kind of can’t,
this is a high, high-functioning individual normally,
and they just sort of can’t function.
They can’t complete simple things like email
or groceries or things for a short while.
It’s almost like a catatonia,
and they refer to it as a dissociative state.
Do you see that in depression?
I mean, we’re speculating here
as to whether or not that involves a cingulate,
but what you’re saying holds a lot of salience for me
in thinking about this example.
Yeah, yeah.
So you see catatonia as an extreme outcome of depression
and sometimes of schizophrenia and other illnesses.
Dissociation is an extreme outcome,
or even in some cases, a less extreme outcome
of PTSD and trauma.
And it’s also a phenomenon that happens naturally
in some people that are highly hypnotizable.
And so if you ask David Spiegel,
he’d say that some of the work that he’s been working on
is around posterior cingulate
and the capacity to dissociate.
But yeah, with our stimulation approach to DLPFC,
dorsal anterior cingulate,
one of the subscales that moved the most
was the dissociative subscale for hypnotizability.
So even in a normal individual,
you see that change in that kind of experience
of dissociation.
I am highly hypnotizable.
David’s hypnotized me a number of times.
In fact, we have a clip of that
on our Huberman Lab channel.
I’ve always, well, always, starting in my early teens,
I started exploring hypnosis.
I’m extremely hypnotizable.
And self-hypnosis or assisted hypnosis.
I don’t know that I ever go into dissociative states.
I’ll try and avoid forcing you
into running a clinical session right now,
but to assess anything like that.
But this brings about something really interesting,
I think, which is I’m aware
that some of the more popular emerging treatments
for depression include things like ketamine,
which is a dissociative anesthetic, is that right?
And my assumption is that as a dissociative anesthetic,
that it leads to dissociative states
where people can sort of third-person themselves
and feel somewhat distanced from their emotions.
I’ve also been hearing that there are emerging treatments,
psilocybin being one of them,
but some other treatments, MDMA, et cetera,
that we’ll parse each of these in detail,
that lead to the exact opposite state
during the effect of the drug,
which is a highly engaged emotionality
and heart rate and sense of self,
and can also lead to relief of depression.
Now, whether or not this, again,
reflects that depression is many conditions
as opposed to just one,
or whether or not somehow tickling,
or in some cases pushing really hard
on the opposite ends of the scale really matter,
I am absolutely fascinated,
and again, also perplexed by this.
Why would it be that a drug
that induces dissociative states
and a drug taken separately
that induces hyper-associative states
would lead to relief of the same condition?
Yeah, no, that’s a great question.
Yeah, so for ketamine,
the level of dissociation appears to be correlated
with the therapeutic effect.
It appears to be necessary,
but not sufficient to produce an antidepressant effect,
and so folks that don’t have any psychological change
from the ketamine or don’t experience any dissociation
typically tend to have less potent
antidepressant effects from ketamine.
We did a study a couple of years ago.
It was really interesting.
So we gave folks naltrexone,
which is an opiate antagonist,
a mu and kappa opiate receptor antagonist,
and we gave the same individuals a pill of that
or a pill of placebo,
and they had no idea which one they were getting.
Was this low-dose naltrexone?
50 milligrams, so it’s a pretty high dose.
Okay.
Yeah, and so we gave a typical ketamine therapeutic dose,
and then we gave 50 milligrams of naltrexone or placebo,
and then in the same individuals,
we gave two infusions,
one with each of those conditions,
and if they had an antidepressant effect,
we waited until they relapsed,
and then we gave them the other condition,
and then we looked to see
what effect of blocking the opioid receptor,
what effect would you see on the antidepressant effect
of blocking the opioid receptor,
with the idea that if ketamine works the way
that a lot of researchers at the time thought
that it completely worked in,
which is the glutamate system,
then you would have no effect of naltrexone,
because naltrexone just interacts with the opiate system.
It doesn’t do anything with any other systems.
Ketamine has a lot of effects over,
you know, it has clear opiate effects in mice
and various ways of looking at that,
and NMDA receptor antagonism and glutamate effects,
and so if it’s just that the glutamate part
is the part driving the antidepressant effect,
you shouldn’t have any difference
in the antidepressant effect between the two conditions.
If, however, the antidepressant effect is primarily,
the opioid properties of ketamine are necessary
for the antidepressant effect,
then you should have a loss of antidepressant effect
during the ketamine plus naltrexone condition
that you observed in the ketamine plus placebo condition,
and what we saw was that there was a dramatic blockade
of the antidepressant effect when naltrexone was present
in the people that had an antidepressant effect
with ketamine plus placebo alone,
and then some friends of mine did a TMS study with pain
and they stimulated over the left dorsolateral
prefrontal cortex and they gave IV naloxone,
which works basically the same way as naltrexone,
and they were able to block the anti-pain effects of TMS
with an opiate blocker, so this idea that,
another kind of convergent point, right,
this idea that the opioid receptor may have a role
in mood regulation, what’s also interesting is
if you look at people that are getting
a total knee operation, a very painful operation, right,
total knee replacement, and you age, sex,
everything matched the individuals
that are going through that,
but you have a group of people that don’t have depression
and a group of people that do have depression,
the presence of depression triples
the oral opioid dose by day four.
That’s required.
That’s required to cover the pain,
but what may be happening is it’s not just treating
physical pain, it may be treating emotional pain as well,
right, at least transiently it seems to have
an antidepressant effect, chronically it seems
to have a very pro-depressant effect,
it can make people treatment resistant,
but it’s an interesting phenomenon,
but yeah, the opioid system seems to be pretty involved,
but what’s interesting there with the ketamine trial
is that we didn’t see any effect on the dissociation,
and so the dissociation was the same each time,
so the psychological effect of what we call the trip
or the kind of dissociative effect
where people are having a psychological phenomenon
from ketamine, that was identical both times,
and so it kind of, it also challenged this idea
that the psychological experience of the psychedelic effect
may be all that’s necessary to produce an effect
and that the pharmacology doesn’t matter
as long as you can achieve that state,
and so we think we pretty clearly debunked that idea
that the underlying pharmacology and the state
seem to be important.
We don’t know for sure if you can,
and a lot of people are working on this,
if you can take out essentially the psychological effect
and still have a drug that works to treat the illness
that you’re trying to target,
and a lot of, there was a mouse study out this week
where they had an LSD analog,
and they were able to see some animal-level data
to suggest that could be true,
but until we figure that out in humans,
it’s kind of to be determined,
but it is curious, right,
being able to kind of use experimental manipulations
to try to separate some of these phenomenon apart
and really understand what’s doing what.
It’s so critical, and it’s so critical
to the other conversation that we’ll surely get to,
which is the progression of psychedelics
from illicit illegal drugs to clinically validated
and presumably at some point either decriminalized
or legal drugs, which has not yet happened,
at least not in the US,
but just to make sure that people are getting this
and how crucial this is,
what we’re really talking about here
is the fact that if somebody takes a multigram dose
of psilocybin or somebody takes MDMA
or they take ketamine and they experience relief
from their trauma, their depression, their addiction,
or any number of the other things
that indeed those compounds have been shown to be useful for
in certain contexts, clinically supported, et cetera,
there’s this gravitational pull to the idea that,
oh, it was the hallucinations,
it was the dissociative state,
it was the feeling of connectedness,
and what we’re really saying is that
while that certainly could be true,
it may be the case that a major source
of the positive shift that occurs after the effect
of the drug is some underlying biology
like shifts in the mu opioid receptor,
a la your experiments with naltrexone,
or a change in the underlying neuromodulation
that had anywhere from nothing to something
to do with the real shift.
And I know there’s a group up at UC Davis
that published a paper in Nature about a year ago,
also looking at, there’s a chemistry lab,
essentially modifying psychedelics
to remove the hallucinogenic properties,
the mood-altering properties,
and actually seeing some pretty impressive effects
in shifts in mood after the drug wears off.
And I know this gets people upset when they hear it,
a lot of people, this gets a lot of people upset, really,
because people think,
oh, no, it’s the intense experience that matters.
But in fact, that may not be the case at all.
In fact, it’s so powerful for people
that sometimes I liken it in my mind to,
you know, it’s like somebody,
it’s like the birth of a new child,
and it’s such an incredible experience,
and then people feel so much connection,
and then they sort of connect the experience
of the actual birth to the connection,
when in fact, that’s true, it turns out,
but there are a bunch of other things happening too
that’s simply the reflection of the fact
that you’re holding a child
and the pheromonal effects, et cetera.
So anyway, I think it’s very important
that these different variables be figured out.
Along those lines, I want to make sure
that before we dive a bit deeper
into ketamine and psilocybin,
that we do touch on a really important topic
that has been in the press a lot lately,
which is SSRIs, selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors,
because we can’t really have a discussion about depression
without talking about SSRIs.
And then I want to circle back to ketamine and psilocybin.
It seems that there are now data
that essentially state that there’s no direct link
between serotonin levels and depression,
although my understanding is that the SSRIs
are powerfully effective for certain forms
of obsessive-compulsive disorder
and may also be effective for treatment of depression,
but it may, again, be, through some effect,
unrelated to serotonin itself.
Is that right?
And how should we think about SSRIs?
Are they useful?
Are they not useful?
What’s going on with SSRIs in your patients
and in other people as well?
Yeah, so that experiment that I described a bit ago
around the naltrexone and ketamine
is the first time I’m aware of
where we were able to essentially eliminate
an antidepressant’s effect by using a second drug
as kind of a blockade.
And it highlights a bigger issue, right?
The issue that we haven’t had a good way
of really understanding how these drugs work.
And so it’s the difference.
I think a lot of the controversy there
is that it’s been difficult, I think,
for folks to see that something can, on one hand, work,
and on the other hand, we don’t know how it works, right?
And so SSRIs clearly work.
Many, many meta-analyses kind of proving that out, right?
That in a subpopulation of individuals,
they achieve great benefit from depression,
for depression, for obsessive compulsive disorder,
for generalized anxiety disorder, panic,
all these things, you can see an improvement
in those symptoms with what we call SSRIs,
or selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors.
The issue there is that these selective serotonin
reuptake inhibitors end up blocking
the reuptake of serotonin, leaving the serotonin
in this kind of in-between between two neurons for a while,
and allowing for more serotonin to kind of be there.
The issue is that they don’t work immediately, right?
So they don’t work like the same day you start taking them.
And that suggests that probably it’s not exactly
the serotonin being in there that’s directly driving it,
that it’s much more likely that it may have some,
say, brain plasticity effects, right?
We know that things like brain-derived neurotrophic factor
get upregulated with chronic oral antidepressant use.
And so that’s kind of the idea,
is that these things work, but what’s powerful,
and I think what the authors of this paper,
it was an extremely controversial paper,
were in part trying to say was that
there’s not a deficit of serotonin.
You’re not born with what people call a chemical imbalance.
And psychiatry has known this.
This is not actually new information to anybody.
It’s kind of a rehashing of a bunch of information
we’ve known for a while now, but in the lay press,
it’s kind of hit in a way that it didn’t seem
to grab attention before with previous publications,
but this idea that this chemical imbalance idea is wrong.
I really think that part’s important
because I think that for a while, I think psychiatry,
what I’ll call psychiatry 1.0, right,
this kind of idea of Freud and psychotherapy
and its origins, it was a lot around your family,
and those experiences, and psychotherapy
kind of going in and correcting,
or helping you to figure out,
or you being able to see, or people hear you
so that you can eventually come to the conclusion
of certain cognitions that aren’t helping you, right?
And there’s a huge importance there,
but there’s a history where things
like the schizophrenogenic mother and all of that,
that was a concept at some point, right?
And so we’ve transitioned from that
to the, for a long time, the chemical imbalance,
which I’ll call psychiatry 2.0,
this idea that there’s something chemically missing,
and I think that the trouble there
for a patient who’s not a physician,
who’s not someone who’s steeped in these sorts of ideas,
who’s more of kind of a person,
kind of average American out there, right,
is that it’s sending a message
of there’s something missing with me,
whether it be my experiences I had no control over
when I was a child, or a chemical in my brain.
What I think’s really powerful with TMS,
really powerful with TMS, and a level even powerful
with the psychedelic story,
is it’s saying something different.
You know, TMS works, and there’s no serotonin
coming in or out of the brain, right?
And we’re doing a rapid form of TMS
that works in one to five days,
so there’s no, it’s very unlikely
that there’s some long-term kind of upregulation
of serotonin that’s driving that.
So our work actually kind of pushes back
on this serotonin hypothesis
as being kind of the center of depression,
because it says, look, we’re not giving anybody
any serotonin, we’re simply turning these brain regions on,
and we’re focused on the circuitry,
and that’s Psychiatry 3.0.
It’s not just like neuromodulation.
Neuromodulation’s a really nice use case for Psychiatry 3.0,
because it’s a way to focally and directly perturb
brain regions in whatever modality you’re using,
but there are a lot of groups
that are actually doing neuroimaging before and after,
and they’re able to see circuit-level changes
for something like psilocybin or ketamine
long after the drug is gone,
suggesting in those same brain regions converge,
so the subgenual default mode network connection
that we see is changing
with our Stanford neuromodulation therapy technique.
It’s that same set of brain regions
that ketamine and psilocybin seem to act on,
act on these connections between brain networks
that seem to shift,
and so it refocuses the story
on something that’s highly correctable,
and it’s basically electrophysiology,
and it’s basically kind of recalibrating a circuit
that is recalibratable,
instead of I have something missing
or I have some set of experiences early in life
that are gonna forever trap me
in these psychiatric diagnoses,
and so it kind of challenges that idea,
and I think that’s what’s so powerful
about Psychiatry 3.0,
this idea of focusing on the circuit
because it gets us into thinking about psychiatry
and psychiatric illnesses as something that are recoverable.
People can get better.
We’ve seen with our TMS techniques,
we’ve seen it with some of the psychedelic work
that we’ve done where people are actually
in normal levels of mood for sustained periods of time.
Within five days.
Within five or less days,
and in the case of the psychedelics,
within a few days, right?
So we can get people out of these states.
They’re totally well.
There’s no drug in their system at that point.
In the case of psychedelics,
there was never a drug in their system
in the case of TMS,
and it just tells us that it’s fixable.
It’s just like the heart.
It’s just like an arrhythmia in the heart.
It’s just like these other illnesses
that it’s like a broken leg.
We can go in and do something
and we can get somebody better,
then I think what’s empowering
and what a lot of patients have told me
is they say,
some people will relapse and need more stimulation
or need more psychedelics or whatever it is,
but they’ll tell me I’ve relapsed
and I’m depressed again,
but I’ll never think about killing myself again
because I know that if I go get stimulated again,
it improves.
It gets better.
I will be able to reach it
and I don’t fear that I’m chronically broken.
I don’t fear that the chemical imbalance
is still imbalanced.
I don’t fear that these things
that I couldn’t control in my childhood
are gonna be there and drive this problem forever
and I think that’s what’s so powerful about this.
The sense of control.
The sense of control.
The sense of,
they’re not doing the stimulation themselves.
They’re not administering the drug
in these trials themselves.
They probably never will.
These will probably be medical treatments,
but they are choosing to do it
and in that sense,
they are in control.
I have a good friend.
I won’t out him for reasons
that will become clear in a moment
who was quite obese
and lost a lot of weight
and was really proud of himself
and then I guess we could say
he sort of relapsed in a sense.
Not all the way,
but far along,
but his tone around it was very different.
He knew he had accomplished his goal once before.
He was disappointed in himself,
but he knew exactly why he had relapsed.
It was very clear.
He had essentially relapsed
to the previous set of eating behaviors
and lack of exercise behaviors
and has now brought himself back again.
And it just resonates with your story
that once somebody understands they can do it
because they’ve been there before.
This idea again of considering new rules.
And that brings me to this question about psychedelics
and the, frankly,
the altered thinking and perception
that occurs in high-dose psilocybin clinical sessions.
It seems that the disordered thinking,
even though it could be random, right?
Hearing colors and seeing sounds
is always the kind of cliche statement
of the Timothy Leary era.
Also, right there, that’s a Stroop task of sorts.
It’s a synesthesia.
It’s a combining of perceptions,
but it’s sort of Stroop task-ish
in that it’s a new set of rules for the same stuff, right?
And many people do report improvements
in trauma-related symptomology and depression,
as I understand it,
from my read of the clinical trials after taking psilocybin,
because during those sessions,
something comes to mind spontaneously.
As you and I were talking about earlier,
they will report, for instance,
a new way of seeing the old problem.
And the old problem could be the voice
that they’re no good, nothing will ever work out,
or it could be even more subtle than that.
So that raises two questions.
One is about the basic functioning of the human brain,
which is, why do you think the brain
would ever hold on to rules that don’t serve us well?
That’s one question.
And then the second question is,
what is it about psilocybin and related molecules
in terms of their neurochemistry,
in terms of the ways they disrupt thinking and feeling,
et cetera, during the session,
that allow this novel rule consideration phenomenon?
Yeah, so the first question,
I think it’s an evolutionary neurobiology answer, right?
I think that at the individual person level,
it doesn’t make a whole lot of sense
that when we’re really stressed out,
some of us want to eat more, right?
At the individual person level,
because that’s not particularly that good
for my health in the long term.
But if you think about it in some 500 years ago,
1,000 years ago, if I’m highly stressed out,
it’s most likely that I’m about to not have food
at some point, and I should eat a bunch of food
that is high-fat, high-sugar, high-carb food
to put on weight for that next phase,
where in this stress, I may be in battle
and I don’t have food and I have enough fuel on board,
right, and so we end up being a result
of probably a lot of biology that’s not that useful
in the modern era, and I think in the brain
for, let’s say, PTSD, right?
A lot of veterans come back
and they experience these PTSD symptoms,
and they’re not at all useful back home, right?
They hear some loud noise and all of a sudden,
they’re behind a car or they’re behind a,
I’ve heard of folks jump and run behind a trash can
or whatever in the middle of San Francisco
when they hear a loud noise,
but if you put them back in the battlefield,
you have that.
That’s highly adaptive, right?
And so I think what’s interesting
is that we, in the absence
of using substances like psychedelics,
we end up having these very persistent memories
that are attached to negatively valence to motion
predominantly, as you were saying earlier,
the jacket in elementary school,
we had various things like that for me, too, right?
You remember these things,
and we hold onto those things
from, I think, an evolutionary neurobiology standpoint,
but what seems to, for whatever reason,
kind of alleviate that are these substances,
some new, like MDMA,
some that have been around for thousands of years,
like psilocybin, and used in kind of sacramental,
and as a sacrament in traditions,
seem to have a therapeutic effect
that seems to be pretty long-lasting for these phenomenon,
and so it’s just curious, right?
It’s curious that in the absence of that,
these things will keep going on and on,
but in the presence of that exposure,
then all of a sudden you see a resolution of the problem,
and we have some work now,
we’re treating folks with Navy SEALs,
and the data’s still being analyzed,
but the anecdotes that we’re getting, right,
are folks are coming back,
and they’re saying it’s finally gone, right?
These set of PTSD symptoms are finally gone,
and so this idea that, for whatever reason,
going into what’s probably a highly plastic state,
like we were talking about earlier,
upregulation of brain-derived neurotrophic factor,
in the case of ibogaine,
glial-derived neurotrophic factor,
this highly plastic state,
and the ability to kind of re-experience memories,
and then, as you know, we always re-consolidate a memory
when we bring it back up.
But re-consolidating it in that state,
for whatever reason, may drive a therapeutic effect.
And, you know, the jury’s still out.
I would say that I’m kind of agnostic
to what tool I’m using kind of guy.
My business is to find treatments that help people,
and so I’m much more pragmatic about it.
If this sort of thing,
which has a lot of cultural baggage,
but if this sort of thing
ultimately ends up being therapeutic,
if we can design trials that convince me and others
that it is, then we should absolutely use it.
And if it doesn’t, then we clearly shouldn’t use it, right?
And I think that’s a big question
the field’s gonna have to work out.
We have a hard time blinding these trials
because the placebo condition
is not easy to pull off, obviously.
A placebo for a psilocybin journey is hard to imagine.
We’ve been thinking about this,
and maybe that ketamine study
that I was talking about earlier,
if we could give people naltrexone and ketamine,
maybe that’s a good sort of placebo condition, right?
Because we know that we can block
any of the actual antidepressant effects of ketamine,
but they still have an experience, you know?
And so that’s one way of doing it,
but thinking about ways to do that
and really kind of proving this out.
And that’s been, yeah, I think that’s been kind of central
to the way I’ve been thinking about this.
But yeah, I think there’s the work that’s been done so far,
the first psilocybin trial,
the first MDMA trial
that was published in Nature Medicine recently.
And what do those generally say?
I mean, that they are effective for a number of people
after one session, two sessions?
What’s sort of the general contour?
Let’s start with psilocybin and MDMA.
Yeah, so MDMA appears to, in one to a few MDMA sessions,
have an anti-PTSD effect that seems to be, you know,
outside of the kind of standard assumed levels
of PTSD improvement that you can observe in individuals
with this level of PTSD, right?
So what we call the effect size,
which is essentially like a measure,
a Cohen’s d effect size is a measure
that allows for you to compare different treatments
to each other for different conditions
that are, you know, agnostic to what the actual illness is.
You know, the effect size is there,
you know, approach effect size is the things
that are pretty effective,
like antacids for heartburn, right?
And you see that with MDMA treatment.
So does that mean that for people that have trauma,
who do a, and again, we’re talking about
in a clinical setting, they take one or two doses of MDMA,
I think the standard MAPS dose is 150 to 775 milligrams,
again, doing this with a physician, et cetera,
control clinical trial, legal.
Yeah, exactly.
They do it once or twice.
And broadly speaking, what percentage of people
who had trauma report feeling significant relief
from their trauma afterward?
It’s about two thirds of people
had a clinically significant change in their PTSD.
That’s impressive.
Which is impressive, right?
And how long lasting was that?
I mean, these trials were ended pretty recently, so.
It appears to last for a while.
In the earlier trials where they followed people out,
it seemed to last for kind of in the year’s range
for some people.
And so it’s, you know, it’s pretty compelling.
Psilocybin, you know, in contrast that with ketamine,
which only on average lasts about a week and a half
for a single infusion.
So it’s a much shorter.
So they have to get repeated infusions of ketamine
every 10 days or so?
Forever?
For some people, or they end up getting like a bunch of doses
for a couple of weeks.
And then for some people that seems to last a while.
You know, that’s where I think the psilocybin story
for depression and the MDMA story for PTSD
seem more interesting to me.
So for psilocybin, what is the rough percentages on,
and this would be relief not from trauma,
but from depression, correct?
Yeah, exactly.
So it’s, you know, in open label studies,
it’s closer to like half to two thirds of people
end up getting better depending upon their level
of treatment resistance.
In the blinded trials, it was more like a third or so
of people, you know, experienced relief.
And this is, you know, this is a press release of the data,
you know, and so it hasn’t, to my knowledge,
it hasn’t been published yet.
And so I’m looking forward to seeing the full paper
on that one.
But it, you know, separated from placebo
and looks, you know, looks pretty good as well.
It looks like it’s, you know, the first of two trials
that need to be done to get this thing approved
for treatment resistant depression.
And so that stuff looks good.
In terms of MDMA, for many years,
it was reported in the popular press
and there was a paper published in Science
that MDMA was neurotoxic,
that it would kill serotonin neurons.
This was what was always said.
Then I saw another paper published in Science
that wasn’t a retraction of the previous paper,
but rather was a second paper in the same group
that essentially admitted that the first time around
they had injected these monkeys,
because with not MDMA, but with methamphetamine,
which is known to be neurotoxic.
So it was kind of a public admittance of oops,
or big, like really big screw up, so oops,
but never a retraction
and then never really a publicly acknowledged correction
in the popular press.
So it seems that in the appropriate dosage range
and with these one or two sessions,
my assumption, and this again is an assumption,
tell me if I’m right or wrong here,
is that MDMA is not neurotoxic for serotonergic neurons
at appropriate doses
and with appropriate sourcing, et cetera.
So it was an interesting study that,
I think the guy’s name is Halpern, last name’s Halpern.
Not Casey Halpern.
Not Casey, different now.
I think Joshua Halpern,
I’m blanking on his first name, but-
Casey Halpern was a guest on this podcast
and is a former colleague of ours at Stanford
who unfortunately we lost to University of Pennsylvania
and maybe someday we’ll bring him back.
Yeah, that’s right.
So this individual received some NIH funding to actually,
NIDA, National Institute for Drug Abuse funding
to explore individuals of the Mormon faith in Utah
of the Mormon faith in Utah who partake in only MDMA.
So the way this works is that MDMA happened
kind of after a lot of the religious documents
were developed and so MDMA isn’t on the prohibited drug list.
The banned substance list.
The banned substance list.
I have some good friends who are LDS.
Yeah, great people.
I do as well.
Just to kind of set of facts, you know?
And so these folks only use MDMA,
but they don’t, they’re not, you know,
the problem with some people using drugs
is they’re polysubstance users, right?
So you can’t say it’s the MDMA
if they’ve also taken other psychedelics
and they’ve taken opiates and they’ve taken cocaine
and you have this picture
where you can’t really tease out that problem.
But with this, right, it was just individuals
that were part of the Mormon faith
and so they were kind of purists
in the sense they only used MDMA
and he confirmed all of that
and it was a brilliant study, right?
Because then he was able to go in
and look at their cognitive profiles
versus individuals of the same geography,
the same faith, all of that,
that happened to not take MDMA
and found there were no neurocognitive differences.
So does that mean that it was not damaging?
It was not damaging.
It’s hard to know because to really do this study well,
you’d have to track these folks down
before they ever took MDMA
and do a pre-post and compare to people that didn’t,
but you know, this is about as good of a study
as you can do given the situation
to be able to check this out.
Additionally, when I was back in Charleston
and working at the Medical University of South Carolina,
one of my mentors there, Dr. Wagner,
was a neuropsychologist at MUSC
and he was also the neuropsychologist
for the early MDMA trials
and so he did all the neurocognitive batteries
for individuals pre-post
and similarly did not see any changes
in neurocognitive profiles in a negative way
and so, you know, there’s data
from experimental patients receiving this,
there’s data from people that are chronic users,
who only take MDMA
and that combination of data suggests
that there’s certainly no apparent risk
in the kind of one to two to three dose range.
It’s probably unlikely that at least, you know,
modest dose exposure over a lifetime
doesn’t appear to have a profound
neurocognitive damaging effect.
Interesting, yeah, I know that sourcing is key
and we’re here, we’re talking about clinical trials
where purity is assured
and, you know, years ago when so-called raves
were really popular, maybe they’re still popular,
never been to one, so I wouldn’t know
if they’re happening or not,
that’s how in the know I am,
but it was clear that, you know,
testing for purity was important
because sometimes the drugs
are made such that there are contaminants
like methamphetamine,
which we know is highly neurotoxic.
I think that one reason why people
think that MDMA might be neurotoxic
is the reported drop in energy
or sort of feeling fatigued for a few days afterward.
I spoke to a physician colleague of ours
who said that that very likely has something to do
with the surge in prolactin
that arrives subsequent to the big dopamine surge
that occurs in MDMA.
And I mentioned that because I know a number of people
talk about serotonin depletion after taking MDMA.
He has in mind that while that could be true,
it’s likely that anytime somebody takes something
or does something where there’s a huge lift in dopamine,
that there’s very likely a huge compensatory increase
in prolactin that follows,
and prolactin has a kind of sedative effect,
numbing effect on mood and libido, et cetera,
that eventually also wears off.
Does that make sense to you as a physician?
Yeah, it makes sense.
I mean, you know, the difference between say MDMA
and psilocybin is that MDMA
is kind of an amphetamine of sorts, right?
So it has effects in dopamine,
and psilocybin’s pretty neutral
and maybe a little bit of dopamine effects,
but kind of much more of a serotonergic-focused drug.
And so, yeah, I think you’re going to see
kind of a different profile after,
and I haven’t heard that story,
but that makes sense to me too.
Since you mentioned psilocybin,
let’s talk a little bit about
the neurochemistry of psilocybin.
As a serotonergic agent,
my understanding is it operates on these,
is it the 5-HT serotonin 2C receptor?
2A.
2A, excuse me, 2A and receptors,
and that I’ve seen a bunch of different reports
in terms of what it’s actually doing to the brain
while people are under the effects of the drug.
And this is important for us to segment out
because there are the effects that happen
while people are under the influence,
and then the more long-lasting effects.
But some of the effects I’ve heard about are, for instance,
and tell me again if these are right or wrong,
that there is increased activation of lateral connections,
sort of broader areas of the brain being co-active
than would normally occur.
Maybe that explains some of the synesthesias,
seeing sounds and hearing colors as the trivial example,
but rule-breaking within the mind.
But then I’ve also heard that perhaps
it’s lack of gating of sensory input.
So normally if I’m looking at something,
I’m not thinking about the sensation in my right toe
unless it’s relevant.
But if I’m thinking about the sensation in my right toe,
I’m generally not thinking about the truck
around the corner.
So we have these attentional spotlights,
but that somehow it creates a more, it adds spotlights.
Yeah, it de-gates the thalamus.
De-gates the thalamus, right,
through the reticular thalamic structure.
So what is the evidence that any of that is true?
And are there other phenomena?
Is there involvement of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex
that we are aware of?
And where I’m really headed here in a few minutes is,
is there a place for combining directed stimulation
of the brain with psychedelics
so that the effects of serotonin could be primarily
within the structures that you know from your work
to be relevant to depression?
But to simplify it first,
what’s going on when one takes psilocybin
and why is it interesting in light of depression?
Yeah, definitely.
So David Nutt and Robin Carhart-Harris’s work
around neuroimaging psychedelics
are kind of some of the first folks to do that work.
And to their great surprise,
they thought there was gonna be an increase
in activity on psychedelics.
And what they found is the opposite, right?
There’s kind of an overall decrease in the level
of activity in the brain with psychedelics.
But they have also looked at connectivity
and there’s this kind of small world,
large world connectivity that you think about.
And so small world meaning there’s a lot,
there’s kind of a much more kind of focused
kind of cortical function or subcortical function
or whatever it is.
And what you see is a difference in that level of engagement
and of brain regions.
So the connectivity, kind of global connectivity
to your point kind of increases.
And so it’s interesting.
I think to kind of have a convergent theory on this,
it’s still to be determined.
There’s still a lot of work I think that needs to be done.
But it’s certainly suggestive
that there’s pretty profound changes in brain activity
and brain connectivity after.
And what we found to be really interesting
is the antidepressant effects of psilocybin
have a particular connectivity change
that we also see with our TMS approaches, right?
And it’s this connectivity
between the subgenual anterior cingulate
and the default mode network.
And so when we do this effective
Stanford neuromodulation therapy stimulation,
we see a down regulation,
the connectivity between the negatively valenced mood state
in the case of depressed individuals
and the self-representation of the brain.
And you see that same connectivity change
occur post psilocybin,
suggesting there’s a convergent mechanism.
And it makes sense, right?
You’ve kind of got an over-connected,
negatively valenced system, conflict system
that’s kind of attached onto the self-representation
and people feel stuck, right?
And then when you do whatever you do that’s effective,
it unpairs those two systems.
I want to ask you about this phenomenon
I’ve heard about during psilocybin journeys.
I heard about this from Dr. Matthew Johnson,
who’s running a lot of the clinical trials at Johns Hopkins
and has been a guest on this podcast.
He said that there’s something seems to be important
about the patient who’s depressed
or who’s and is under the influence of psilocybin
or the patient who’s trying to get over smoking
or an eating disorder,
who’s taking psilocybin and is in the clinic,
that there’s something important to this notion
of letting go,
that people will feel as if their thoughts
and their feelings and maybe even their body
aren’t under their control
and that the clinician’s job under those circumstances
is of course to make sure that they’re physically safe,
that they don’t jump out a window
or try to actually give an example of a patient
who thought that, I think it was a she,
could move into the painting in the wall.
And obviously that wasn’t true in the real world,
although it was true in her mind.
So they prevented her from doing that.
But that letting go,
that somehow untethering from the autonomic arousal
that’s occurring is important,
which brings us back to this idea
or me back to this idea of like a seesaw
where you’re sort of letting go of the hinge
and just sort of your heart rate’s going up,
like just go with it and trust.
Your heart rate’s going down, just go with it and trust.
You’re thinking about something very powerful
and depressing related to your childhood.
You’re just supposed to go there without fear.
You’re thinking about what’s possible
in terms of what could happen.
So anyway, you get the picture.
Can we think of that as just the willingness
to do a million different variations
on the emotional stroop task?
You will entertain the full array of rules within your head
and consider them.
Or is there something more to it?
And again, we’re in the outer margins
of understanding here,
but what are your thoughts on this notion of letting go
as such a key variable for relief from depression
during the psychedelic journey?
Yeah, so I’ll talk a little bit about something
called exposure and response prevention therapy
that’s a typical kind of gold standard treatment for OCD.
And I’ll help this a little bit conceptually.
And so what that really is, is it’s a letting go therapy.
And so exposure response prevention,
the idea is that you have to expose the individual
to something that triggers an obsession
that they then want to do whatever the compulsion is.
And so I’ll give you my first exposure
and response prevention patient when I was a resident.
He was very concerned about leaving the lights on his car.
And so what we did is we went out
and we turned the lights on in his car
and locked his door so his lights were on.
And he was super worried this is gonna kill his battery.
And we went and we spent an hour talking about things.
And we went back out to his car and his battery was fine
and his lights were on and he cranked the car
and we did it maybe one other time.
And then all of a sudden that was gone, right?
And that’s the idea, is that you’re essentially exposing
and you want to do it at levels that are,
from an anxiety standpoint, tolerable.
But exposing the person to something
and then letting them see that that exposure
ends up being fine, right?
It ends up not causing the thing
that they end up being worried about.
And so in some sense, being in the psychedelic state,
and we’re all taught at a level
to retain some level of control.
People have more or less of that,
but we’re all effectively retaining some level of control.
We all wake up in the morning
and put clothes on to go into society.
We all try to say, most people try to say the right things.
They don’t try to do things that are outside
of cultural norms when they’re in conversation.
And so we’re constantly at some level
controlling the situation that we’re in.
And so it’s not, it makes a lot of sense
that in that state, part of the therapeutic effect
that may be linked to the neural circuitry
is this idea of letting go
and essentially letting the system,
the network configuration maybe, whatever it is,
assume a state that you’ve essentially
been fighting the whole time.
It’s the same way that my OCD patient
was fighting this need to click the off button
on the lights of his car 50 times
before he would go and do whatever he needed to do.
And at some level, letting go there,
meaning letting us just turn the lights on
and him not do anything, or letting go,
meaning in the psychedelic state,
you’re just letting go of whatever it is
you’re holding onto, negatively valenced thoughts
about yourself in the setting of having depression
or re-experiencing a trauma, memory,
and allowing that to just happen
and re-seeing it again through a different light.
It feels the same in the sense that
that’s allowing for whatever’s going on
with these psychedelic states to do whatever they do.
It’s fascinating.
You said it’s exposure response therapy
is the traditional name?
Exposure response prevention therapy.
Done outside of the psychedelic journey.
It’s done outside the psychedelic journey,
but that idea of letting go is present in both of those.
Psychotherapy, straight up, totally sober,
non-psychedelic, non-anything,
manualized psychotherapy that we know works really well
for OCD and then in that psychedelic state.
And so people have done studies with psilocybin
and now there’s some studies with MDMA
trying to look at treating OCD
with the same sort of idea of letting go, right?
And how do you have an OCD patient kind of let go,
maybe even letting go of not washing their hands anymore,
kind of accepting the idea
they’re not going to get germs on their hands
or whatever it is.
And so it’s kind of part and parcel
of that same sort of thinking.
When I was in college,
I developed a compulsive superstition.
I’m not afraid to admit this.
I somehow developed a knock on wood superstition
and I was actually kind of ashamed of it
because it rationally made no sense.
I don’t consider myself a superstitious person,
never was a superstitious kid.
I’d step on the sidewalk cracks,
I’d walk under ladders,
I’d probably even try to walk under a ladder,
even though I don’t suggest it.
But somehow I picked this thing up
and I used to sneak it at times.
I told my girlfriend at the time that I had it
in hopes that that would prevent me from doing it.
And it’s tricky.
Sometimes it actually comes back where I think,
gosh, I didn’t say knock on wood,
I didn’t knock on wood,
I hope that doesn’t actually happen.
And it’s quote unquote crazy, right?
But crazy in the sense that it makes no sense rationally
why the events would be linked.
And yet I think a lot of people out there
do have internal superstitions.
Maybe by talking about it now, it’ll go away.
Clearly I just need to challenge it.
You know, it’s a, anyway,
I mentioned it because I consider myself,
you know, generally rational person,
but it’s interesting how these motor patterns
get activated and this notion of letting go,
because I don’t actually know what consequence I fear.
And the fear, as I was hearing the example you gave,
you know, the fear of the car battery running down,
I was about to say,
well, what if the battery actually did run out,
then the therapy would be undermined.
And yet that could also be interesting too,
because it’s not that big of a deal.
You jump the car.
But in my case,
I need to think about what the ultimate fear is.
Yeah, and you know, I think a lot of people,
so there’s, it’s interesting if you look at,
say the OCD scale or the depression scale or whatever,
we don’t define normal as zero.
We define normal as some number range above,
so zero to, in the case of the Montgomery Asperg
depression rating scale,
one of the depression scales we use, 10, right?
That’s the normal range.
And so people can have some sadness
and still be considered normal.
In the case of the OCD scale,
it’s about the same 10, right?
Where we say it’s kind of starts to be,
you know, mildly abnormal or something.
And I always, you know,
I’d always tell the medical students,
look, my friends that are surf instructors,
they’re more like a zero on the YBOC.
People that are professionals, you know,
they’re non-zero, but it’s still within the normal range.
And especially, you know,
in the case that you’re talking about,
it doesn’t sound like it got in your way.
You’re obviously highly successful,
tenured professor at Stanford
and do all the great things that you do.
And so it’s very much kind of within the normal range.
And I think totally assumed that a lot of people
have these sorts of things.
And as long, I think something as a psychiatric diagnosis,
when it severely impairs your ability to function,
and that’s when we kind of cross that threshold.
But, you know, I think that a lot of people,
and it’s great that you’re bringing this up.
I mean, it’s very anti-stigmatizing
that you’re bringing it up, right?
Because I think a lot of people hold that stuff in
and they don’t want to talk about it
because they’re worried
that somebody else may think something.
But the reality is, as a psychiatrist,
I talk to a lot of patients,
a lot of people that are, you know, family members,
you know, folks that are just going through
a death in the family, whatever it is.
And what you figure out is like,
everybody’s got a little something here and there.
Everybody has the knock in some way, if that makes sense.
And it’s just, and we’re just all,
we’re all just kind of more predisposed
not to talk about it.
But I think it’s important to talk about it
because I think that when we start all talking about it,
then we realize that we’re all
kind of in this together in a way.
And that we’re, and then some folks that, you know,
have to knock 100 times, we call that OCD.
When, you know, and they have all, you know, germ,
they’re worried about germs and all these other things,
we call that OCD.
And then in that circumstance, you know,
they need treatment, right?
But it is really on, just like blood sugar,
just like blood pressure, it’s on a range, you know,
and it’s not just these discrete diagnoses.
You have them or you don’t.
And it’s good to know.
I actually feel some relief just hearing this
because I am slightly, I wouldn’t say ashamed,
I was sort of embarrassed by it,
but I offer it as a, you know,
it is what it is, as they say.
And it certainly doesn’t seem to hinder my life
as much, knock on wood.
So if we could talk a bit about Ibogaine.
I don’t know much about Ibogaine,
although anytime I hear the, you know, A-I-N-E,
you know, lidocaine, Ibogaine, I think of an anesthetic
and going to the dentist,
which is an unpleasant experience for me, generally.
What’s, what is Ibogaine?
Does this have anything to do with the so-called toad?
You know, people talk about smoking frog skin, toad skin.
What is it used for clinically?
Is it legal in the U.S. as a clinical tool?
Who’s using it and for what purposes?
If you could educate me on Ibogaine,
I truly know nothing about it,
except I think I know how to spell it correctly.
Yeah, that’s fair.
So Ibogaine is one of the alkaloids
that you can extract from a Iboga tree root bark
that’s typically growing in the country of Gabon, Africa.
So Gabon is one of the West African countries,
kind of middle of Africa on the West Coast.
And Gabon is, has a group of ferns
or folks, you know, called the Bwiti.
It’s a religious kind of sacramental group
that sacramentally uses Iboga root bark
as part of that, the sacrament.
And they, they’ve been using Iboga root bark
for a very long time.
And it’s, it’s, you know, part of the tradition.
There’s a whole, there’s a whole set of,
of kind of ceremony around it.
If you’re interested in this,
there’s a book called Breaking Open the Head
by Daniel Pinchbeck that goes,
goes through and talks about this, this whole process.
But essentially, the Gabonese have been using this
for a long time.
And it’s a, it’s a kind of an atypical psychedelic.
It’s not, it’s not a psychedelic
that we normally think about with psilocybin and LSD,
where there are visual perceptual changes, right?
So if you, if you take psilocybin or LSD,
what you, what you experience is you experience
these kind of visual perceptual differences
in the external world, right?
And on enough LSD or psilocybin,
an individual can actually perceive something visually
in the external world that isn’t there,
as we talked about earlier.
Ibogaine doesn’t do that.
Ibogaine does something different.
It’s kind of like, have you ever seen Minority Report
with, you know, the movie with Tom Cruise?
I think 15 or 20 years ago or something,
so it dates us a little bit.
But it was this, it was this movie
where he would be able to go and see these kind of pre-crimes
and he had this big, you know, this big screen
where he could look at scenes from, from time
and like kind of go through that scene and see it.
And so what, what individuals taking Ibogaine will say
is that open eyes, they don’t see anything,
but closed eyes, they’ll go back through
and re-experience earlier life memories
and they will be able to experience it
from a place of empathy, not only for themselves,
but from others and kind of a detached empathy
and being able to see this as almost a third party,
even though they were there,
but they’re able to see it, you know, as a third party.
So Claudio Naranjo, a psychiatrist from Argentina,
you know, described this in a lot of books that he wrote
in I think the 80s and 90s around this.
And so, you know, Ibogaine’s been around for a long time.
Howard Lotsoff, American guy that brought it over
from Africa, he was a polysubstance user,
used every drug, you know, that he had his hands on,
took Ibogaine, including a lot of other psychedelics,
by the way, took Ibogaine
and then never did another drug again, supposedly,
because he had such a profound Ibogaine experience.
Ibogaine is in no way a recreational substance.
It’s not a recreational substance
if you want it to be a recreational substance,
because you’re essentially having this,
what they call a life review.
They also call it 10 years of psychotherapy in a night.
So these are the terminology that people talk about.
The issue-
How long does it last?
Is it truly one night?
It’s usually, you know, it can go,
depending upon if you get re-dosed or anything,
go sometimes, depending upon how fast you metabolize it,
sometimes 24, sometimes 36 hours.
Sometimes it can be shorter, but it is a long time.
It’s a very long time.
So it’s definitely the longest acting
psychedelic substance I know of.
And so people will take this,
and they’ll have this re-evaluation of a given memory,
and then, as we were talking earlier,
re-consolidate that memory again,
and then it seems to have an effect
of that re-consolidation process.
And so about four or five years ago,
I was tapped by Robert Malenko,
one of the senior neuroscientists we both know
in the university.
He says, well, there’s an unnamed donor
that’s very interested in funding a group,
you know, a scientific kind of open-label study
of these Navy SEALs that have been going down to Mexico
and taking Ibogaine, and also a 5-MeO-DMT,
which I’ll talk about in a second,
to treat PTSD, you know,
they claim to have traumatic brain injury,
depression, you know, that whole constellation of symptoms.
You know, and as it was described to me
by various people that had done this,
by their spouses and whatnot, you know,
John, we’ll just say John,
John couldn’t screw a light bulb into a light fixture,
right, they were just so debilitated,
they couldn’t do simple tasks,
what we call activities of daily living.
And they were coming back
and having these really dramatic improvements
in, you know, all aspects of life.
And so, you know, we have over the last couple of years
been able to do this first in human
kind of full neurobiological,
clinical neurocognitive evaluation
of what Ibogaine is doing.
In this case, in special operations,
special forces individuals, former Navy SEALs,
former Army Rangers, that kind of crew of folks,
and look at the pre-post changes that we,
that they’re experienced to be able
to totally quantitate all of that.
And so we’ve been able to capture
all the clinical scales, you know,
depression scales, PTSD scales,
all that standard stuff.
Neurocognitive batteries,
so how does your executive function work specifically,
how does your verbal memory, all of that.
And then neuroimaging and EEG.
So this will be the first human study of Ibogaine for those.
And the reason why is because Ibogaine’s kind of the,
both seemingly the most potent and most,
seemingly to me at least, most powerful psychedelic,
but the one that has the most risk too,
because it has a cardiac effect.
It seems to be that you can screen people out
that have risk off of their electrocardiogram
and reduce the risk quite a bit,
and that’s what we all did.
But that’s why people haven’t really studied it as much.
And it isn’t as, in addition,
there’s no, nobody goes to a rave on Ibogaine.
There’s no recreation at all with it.
It’s not fun.
It’s, people say that it’s relieving,
but it’s hard work, right?
Because, yeah, you’re reexamining things.
And, you know, and so then,
so then we see these folks after,
and I’ll tell you, you know,
we haven’t fully analyzed the data yet,
but I’ll tell you that, you know,
from what my folks are telling me,
it’s pretty dramatic.
You know, people come back and they’re doing,
they’re doing a lot better.
They’re doing a lot better,
and nobody, I’ll knock on wood,
nobody’s had any sort of cardiac issue at all,
you know, in the cohort that we’ve studied.
And they look a lot better,
and they feel a lot better too.
And they describe these experiences
of being able to go back through,
and, you know, soldiers experience
something called moral injury, right?
Where they, maybe they accidentally blew something up
and it had a kid in it, or something like that.
You know, if they’re in Afghanistan or Iraq,
maybe a child died on accident,
or maybe, you know, a civilian died,
or whatever it was, right?
And then they suffer these moral injuries
as part of the job,
and it’s almost one of the kind of, you know,
vocational risks.
They come back and say that they’ve forgiven themselves,
which is huge, right?
And part of that is being able to see themself
in a different light,
and having empathy, finally, for themself,
and being able to kind of have that experience of forgiving.
And so, very cool.
The study, you know, what was happening
was they were taking Ibogaine,
and then taking something called 5-MeO-DMT,
people call it TOAD.
It’s the Sonoran River TOAD, I think.
It’s like, you can find these in Mexico,
find them in Arizona.
And in the back of the TOAD,
produces something called 5-MeO-DMT,
which is a flavor of DMT
that produces a particular psychedelic effect,
also used as a sacrament.
Is it a dimethyltryptamine, is that?
It is a 5-MeO-dimethyltryptamine.
So it’s a kind of dimethyltryptamine
with a kind of addition to it.
The deal there is that it lasts longer
than traditional DMT.
You know, it’s like 20 minutes instead of five,
three or whatever kind of thing.
And so then, so these guys were taking Ibogaine,
and then they would take the 5-MeO-DMT
after we had to kind of divorce those two things,
be able to do the study
and just understand what Ibogaine was doing.
And they go back down a month later,
and they’ll do the 5-MeO-DMT.
So two completely separate sessions.
Two completely separate sessions.
And then one quick question about Ibogaine
before a bit more on 5-MeO-DMT.
Is the Ibogaine journey guided,
or the person just closes their eyes
and they just start falling
into the back catalog of memories?
They have a bunch of preparatory sessions,
and then they have a bunch of sessions after
that they kind of, they’re able to kind of rehash things.
During, there’s a sitter that sits there
and kind of sits with them and helps them out.
But it’s not, it’s pretty,
the phenomenon of the drug seems to drive a lot of this.
Right?
And so a lot of it ends up being
what we call supportive psychotherapy,
or just kind of being there.
And you know, maybe you’re holding the person’s hand,
maybe you’re just saying, I’m here,
or maybe whatever it is,
but you’re making sure they know you’re around.
But you’re not really,
there’s not really an interaction per se.
And then the whole kind of goal there
is just to get folks to kind of go back through
and re-examine these memories
and ultimately look like they re-consolidate them.
And you know, it’s very interesting.
I mean, there’s this kind of, as you said earlier,
Timothy Leary kind of sociocultural construct
that ends up being overlaid over psychedelics.
And what I think is that if you rid yourself
of all of those preconceived notions
of what it is and isn’t and the counterculture movement,
all that stuff that neither of us were ever involved in,
neither of us were ever partaken,
it’s kind of straight scientists looking at this, right?
If you can kind of rid yourself
of all those sociocultural constructions
and then re-examine this,
these, if we just discovered these today,
we would say that these sorts of drugs
are a huge breakthrough in psychiatry
because they allow for us to do
a lot of the sorts of things we’ve been thinking about
with SSRIs, with psychotherapy,
but kind of combined, right?
Psychotherapy plus drugs in a substance
that kind of allows you to re-examine these things.
And so it’s interesting, there’s a lot to do
to try to figure out if that’s true, you know?
And I can say that as it stands right now,
we don’t know if that statement is true, right?
There’s a lot more work that needs to happen
for that statement to be proven to be true.
But the hypothesis is if it is true,
then it’s very likely that this will be seen
as a breakthrough because it allows you
to do these sorts of things that you can’t do
with normal waking consciousness.
But also why we have to really think about this
and you know, these drugs can’t be recreational drugs.
They really shouldn’t be recreational drugs, right?
They’re really too powerful to be used
in the context of recreation
because they can put you into these states.
And this generation of psychedelic researchers
are really clear about that.
You know, I think the 60s folks were not clear about that
and they felt like there was this whole kind of cultural
thing that was going on there.
But I think this cohort of individuals really understands
that in order to really make this happen,
we have to understand that if you need a prescription
for an SSRI, which doesn’t change your consciousness
a whole lot, and we’re very worried about that
and the doctor has to evaluate you for that every week,
that the idea that some of these substances
would go outside of very strict medical supervision
is kind of preposterous, actually.
It’s kind of a dumb moment, I think,
for all of medicine to say, look,
if we’re gonna do this right, we’ve gotta do it
in such a way that’s so protected, that’s so safe,
that we make sure people know
these things are not recreational
and they’re really for the pure purposes
of really powerfully changing cognition for a while
and letting people have these, what seem to be,
you know, relatively therapeutic states.
I think it’s great that you’re doing this study
and along the lines of the sort of early iterations
of psychedelics and the counterculture of the 60s and 70s,
some of which took place, like One Flew Over the Cuckoo’s
Nest, I think, is actually based on the Menlo Park VA,
which is, you know, in our neighborhood of Stanford.
And things are quite a bit different now.
I know you and I have spent some time
with the operators and former operators
at an event on Last Veterans Day, in fact,
the so-called Veterans Solutions Group
that’s pioneering a lot of these psychedelic treatments
for former special operators and current special operators.
And what’s interesting to me about that
is in contrast to the counterculture movement
of the 60s and 70s, that room was filled with people
that are very much of a structure, the military, right?
So it’s no longer considered left-wing, right-wing,
anti-military, pro-military.
Here, this is just about one group of people
who’s exploring psychedelics as a treatment
for trauma and PTSD and other things.
And of course, you also have other domains of society
looking at this.
And in fact, it was really interesting
because there were both far left and far right politicians
at that event, up on stage together,
talking about, in kind of lighter terms, heart medicine,
but also talking about neurobiology.
And it was just fascinating from the perspective
of somebody who’s trying to learn about this stuff
that psychedelic therapies no longer sit
within the anti-establishment realm.
It’s independent of all that.
Certainly when people in the military
are adopting it as a potential treatment.
Again, still under exploration,
but also under exploration at universities
like Stanford and Johns Hopkins and UCSF
and University College London and on and on.
Along the lines of tree barks and toad skins,
tell me about ayahuasca and as a plant, it’s intriguing.
And is it a pro-serotonergic drug like psilocybin?
And is it useful for the same sorts of conditions
that we’ve talked about thus far?
And if you could perhaps tell me a little bit also
about the Brazilian prisoner study.
Yeah, yeah, definitely.
Ayahuasca is another psychedelic.
It’s used as a sacrament in Brazil
and in Peru and Ecuador and Colombia.
So a lot of the South American countries.
And what they do is they combine two plants together
where one plant of the two plant combination
would effectively do nothing.
But the two plant combination together
is capable of producing this very profound
psychedelic effect.
And what’s really kind of curious
is that there are, as I understand it,
10 to 20,000 plant species in the Amazon.
And somehow, somebody.
Someone tried them all.
Combined these two plants together
in certain proportionality and cooked this
for five, 10 hours to the point where you cook out
the dimethyltryptamine out of one of the plants
and cook out the reversible monoamine oxidase inhibitor
out of the other plant in such a way
that the reversible monoamine oxidase inhibitor
prevents the GI breakdown of the dimethyltryptamine
in such a way that it’s then allowed
to cross the blood-brain barrier and get into the brain.
And if you didn’t add the reversible
monoamine oxidase inhibitor plant-derived
into this combination, then it would never cross the brain.
If you put people on a standard psychiatry-prescribed
monoamine oxidase inhibitor that wasn’t reversible,
you’d throw them into serotonin syndrome, right?
So this kind of sweet spot that somehow
ayahuasca practitioners have found
being able to get DMT into the brain
from an oral source with this combination
of a monoamine oxidase inhibitor is curious.
And so that substance has been explored
as an antidepressant agent,
and some studies have looked at that.
It also seems to be very safe.
There was a psychiatrist down at UCLA Harbor
who’s done a lot of work with this
where he’s looked at children, even,
that have been exposed to small doses of ayahuasca
as kind of a sacrament within Amazonian tribes
and found no neurocognitive effects,
no neurocognitive effects in adults.
And so it appears to be safe.
It’s kind of part and brought into various religions,
including kind of merged with Catholicism in South America,
which is kind of very interesting.
And so in some sects of Catholicism in Brazil,
it’s used as a sacrament during religious ceremonies.
And so it became interesting to Brazilian researchers
as to whether or not they could affect recidivism rates
for prisoners in Brazilian prisons, right?
So they gave half of the prisoners
some sort of inert substance
and half of the prisoners an ayahuasca session.
And the recidivism rate or the return to prison rate
in the ayahuasca-exposed individuals
was statistically significantly lower
than the recidivism rate in the control group,
suggesting that whatever’s going on there
seems to have an effect on whatever drives
criminal behavior, whatever criminal behavior
that happened to be.
And I don’t have the details
on the exact nature of the crime.
I am also in no way saying that we should just be giving
psychedelics to folks in prison and all of that.
I think that that is a very edgy thing to do
and probably not something that anybody should try.
But it does kind of bring up this curious question
of what is it about that that would drive people
to change those behaviors?
And why do people make those behavioral decisions?
And a lot of times, if you look at prisons
in the United States, people say this,
what’s the biggest mental health facility
in the United States?
It’s a prison.
Yeah, there’s a lot to unpack there, for sure.
The homeless issue, the prison issue.
It does lead to something that I heard recently,
which is related to all this, which is cannabis.
We hear a lot nowadays about,
people will say, well, it’s safer than alcohol.
And we did an episode on alcohol
that at least by my read of the literature,
indeed, alcohol does seem to be quite bad
for our health beyond.
I think it’s pretty clear that not drinking
is better for your health than drinking at all.
And here, I’m not trying to tell people what to do,
but those are what the data say.
And forget the studies on red wine.
You’d have to drink so much red wine
to get enough resveratrol.
It’s not even clear resveratrol does anything useful
in any way, et cetera, et cetera.
Nonetheless, cannabis is now available
in a lot of very high potency forms.
People are vaping cannabis.
People are smoking cannabis.
I certainly am not saying that cannabis is bad
for people necessarily, although I think children,
I would hope that their brain development
would be completed first, get to age 25.
I know that sounds late for a lot of people,
but the THC obviously taps into some endogenous systems
of endocannabinoid systems and is powerful.
And I’ve seen this report that was in Lancet Psychiatry
this last year that said that early use of potent cannabis,
meaning age 14 to 20 or so,
can potentially lead to an exacerbation
of psychosis later in life.
And I actually put this out on social media
and it sort of exploded.
I didn’t expect it to.
And people were saying, well, that’s not causal.
And obviously it’s not causal because people say,
well, maybe people with psychotic tendencies
are seeking out cannabis.
Although that’s sort of a weak argument
in the sense that there’s at least a four times,
a 4X increase in these psychotic episodes
for people later in life.
But what are your thoughts about cannabis?
Because I do want to acknowledge
that it does have medical benefits for certain things,
a pain, chemotherapy.
So by no means trying to knock on cannabis
and its appropriate medicinal use,
but what should we think about cannabis
in terms of this finding that can exacerbate a psychosis
in certain individuals?
Yeah, so I think there’s a couple of things, right?
So cannabis is multiple cannabinoids, right?
THC, CBD, CBN, sativas, and indicas.
It gets, yeah, there’s a lot there to unpack.
Yeah, there’s a lot of,
but there are two main kind of chemicals you think about
and kind of how things are essentially bred, right?
And so there’s a lot of cannabis
that’s really bred to be very high, very potent THC,
and there’s cannabis where the THC’s bred completely out.
So there’s stories from Colorado, right?
This strain of cannabis that’s THC-free.
There’s no THC at all, and it’s all CBD,
and it’s called Charlotte’s Web.
And a bunch of kids’ parents,
one kid and then kind of a string of parents
after that moved to Colorado when cannabis was legalized
because CBD is anti-epileptic.
So CBD is also anti-psychotic.
And so there have been a number of studies
that if you give CBD at high doses,
it’s anti-psychotic in schizophrenic,
established schizophrenic patients.
The issue is that we’ve bred CBD out of marijuana
selectively over time.
And we’ve gotten very good
at figuring out how to do that, right?
Conversely, THC is pro-psychotic and pro-epileptic, right?
And so when you talk about does cannabis cause psychosis
or does cannabis treat psychosis,
it appears to be more related to the proportions of CBD
to THC than it does to the kind of idea of cannabis.
So for me, there’s a, and I have no stock on this
or anything like that,
but there’s a company called GW Pharmaceuticals
and I haven’t looked into them in a while,
but they have a lot of clinical trials
for something called Dravet’s syndrome,
which is a seizure disorder where kids seize a whole lot.
Lennox-Gastaut syndrome, which is a seizure disorder
where kids are seizing 300 times a day.
Both of these are like kids are seizing so much
they’re basically in a seizure
or in the post-ictal phase constantly.
And they’ve failed everything.
They’ve failed barbiturates, they’ve failed bromides,
which we just don’t use anymore,
except in these cases because of the side effects,
and they’ll give kids CBD.
And I think CBD is a pretty safe drug
compared to bromide, right?
And so this idea that CBD in a kid is actually safe.
It’s a cannabinoid, but it’s CBD and it’s safe, right?
And so that to me is totally fine.
Also giving CBD as an adjunctive treatment for schizophrenia
there’ve been some positive trials
and negative trials in that,
but there seems to be no negative side effects.
It seems to reduce some of the metabolic syndrome issues
in folks with schizophrenia who are having side effects
from the primary antipsychotic.
The converse is there’s clearly cases
where people that are taking very high doses of THC
become psychotic, they get put into the psychiatric unit,
nothing happens other than they kind of get the THC
out of their system,
and then they resolve their psychosis, right?
And so that, and a handful of people
who’ve had seizures related to high doses of THC,
and syncope, and all sorts of things.
And so this idea that THC, high doses of THC
can be pro-psychotic is also not taking a shot
at people that think that cannabis overall is a good thing.
It’s just, it just is what it is.
And the kind of pure, if you,
I think if you zoom back and you say,
you’re a true naturalist,
you’re thinking about natural medicines in the world,
you should think, well, probably marijuana
was balanced THC, CBD at some point,
and then we just, we humans messed with it, right?
And that most likely that was probably okay at some level,
and then we pushed it one way or another.
And what I mean by okay is in a 45-year-old,
it’s okay kind of thing.
Now, what I think is going on with the kids,
with the teenagers is you’ve got prefrontal maturation,
right, and then you’re exposing them
to a whole lot of high THC load.
And while it’s unclear if it’s cause or effect,
it’s certainly in the picture.
And if I were a parent,
I wouldn’t want my 16-year-old smoking marijuana.
If I were a parent and my 30-year-old
otherwise healthy, totally fine, whatever,
banker, lawyer, kid, decided to try marijuana
for the first time, I wouldn’t scold them about it, right?
So I think it’s kind of a different thing, right?
I would never want my up to 25-year-old,
just like you’re saying, before prefrontal maturation,
I would never want my kid to be exposed at all.
But it looks like, except in susceptible individuals
or susceptible to drug-induced psychosis,
it looks like it’s a relatively safe thing
past prefrontal maturation.
Again, I’m not gonna comment of cause and effect,
but I would say that if you’re a parent,
it doesn’t make much sense, right?
You never know what’s ultimately gonna hurt your kid.
I mean, we were talking about this earlier,
my wife’s pregnant now, she kind of avoids everything,
right, and rightfully so, right?
This idea that we just, we wanna be careful
when our children’s brains are developing,
and I think that’s really what you were saying,
and I think actually important.
The bigger question that you asked,
which is relative risks of drugs, is an interesting one.
So David Nutt published in, I think it was in The Lancet,
I’ll have to look it up, but I think in The Lancet,
an article about relative drug risks
for the person and for society, and this was like,
he was on the UK’s British drug policy group,
where essentially what he showed was,
if you look at societal risk plus personal risk,
and you combine those two, you know what drug
is the most dangerous drug in the world?
I’m gonna guess it’s alcohol.
It’s alcohol, right behind heroin and cocaine,
and da-da-da-da-da, and somewhere in the middle
is marijuana, and right on the tail end,
on the other, on the exact other end of this, psilocybin.
Is caffeine, usually doesn’t make the list.
It may have been on the list.
If it was, it was probably pretty close to psilocybin,
but somewhere in the middle was ketamine,
somewhere in the middle was amphetamine,
somewhere in the, you know, a little closer to psilocybin,
I think it was MDMA, you know, but it’s this combined
personal, you know, kind of world risk of these things,
and so alcohol makes it, because there’s a huge amount
of personal risk, and there’s a huge amount
of societal risk, right?
Drunk drivers kill X amount of people in the world.
Fight, sexual assault, all that.
Yeah, and then all the cancer and all that stuff,
and so it beats out cocaine, it beats out heroin,
it beats out all of these things, and yet we don’t,
we don’t as a culture, for whatever reason,
we don’t as a culture see it as a drug,
and that’s the part that really baffles me, you know?
And I’m not-
I mean, they serve it, I mean, this is no knock
on Stanford at all, of course I wouldn’t do that.
This is, at every institution I’ve been to,
they serve alcohol at the graduate student events.
That’s right.
You know, they serve alcohol, they do a happy hour.
I’ve never been a drinker, I can take it or leave it,
so, and I realize that some people,
they really enjoy alcohol, you know, my former partner,
I mean, she just was in that, you know, 10% or so
of people who have a glass of wine and just feel great,
and the second one feel great.
I just want to take a nap after I have a bit of alcohol,
so it never does much for me.
I always feel poisoned, I feel lucky in that sense,
but it’s unbelievable that it is so prevalent,
and it’s just, it’s baked into the medical,
even medical institutions, they’ll pop a bottle of champagne
to celebrate the opening of a hospital.
That’s right, that’s right.
You know, it’s pretty crazy.
Yeah, no, you’re absolutely right.
You know, I think what’s going to happen,
but this is me, you know, looking at a crystal ball
a little bit, but I think what’s going to happen
is what happened with doctors and smoking.
So if you look at the 50s and 60s, right,
there are all these pictures of doctors smoking cigarettes,
you know, with patients or, you know,
psychiatrists doing psychotherapy and smoking a cigarette
with the patient sitting on the couch, you know,
surgeons smoking a cigarette in between cases.
There are all these pictures of that, right,
and now all of a sudden, smoking’s totally banned.
I think it’s totally banned for most of Stanford campus.
My suspicion is, as you’re suggesting, right,
you know, this is everywhere and it’s all kind of ubiquitous.
At some critical point, some tipping point,
everybody’s going to realize that just like with smoking,
we’ve got to rid hospital systems
and universities of alcohol.
And at some point in 50 years, it’s my view that
we’ll look back at these scenarios that you’re talking about
and be like, you know what, we were foolish about this.
We can’t believe that we gave people alcohol
when they graduated from whatever, you know,
and I think we’ll have a different take on it.
But it’s going to take a longer time.
I think people did a really good job
tying smoking to lung cancer,
and it’s like a very simplistic story,
smoking, lung cancer, you know?
Now, as you know, alcohol increases the risk
of a lot of different cancers,
but it’s not so clear which one.
I mean, there’s like, you know, the kind of oral,
like the throat, tongue cancer, that’s one of them.
Breast cancer.
Yeah, breast cancer, you know,
and so it’s kind of, it’s a harder story to tell,
you know, and I think that’s why, and everybody,
you know, and then there’s this whole,
my mom says this, it’s like,
I drink my glass of wine because my doctor told me
it was heart healthy, and we were talking about this,
and I try to, no, no, no, but Dr. So-and-so
said it’s heart healthy, and so it ends up being this thing
where, like, she’s drinking alcohol
because she thinks that it’s good for her heart,
and, you know, and it’s hard.
I’ve had those conversations with her.
It’s hard to untie that, and I think that, yeah,
at some point, we’re going to hit some threshold moment,
and it’ll be interesting if we really look at the data,
and we really look at what’s safe and not safe
from purely from this analysis.
It kind of points to the right direction.
It’s really interesting, and also,
just saying nothing of poor judgment
under the influence of alcohol.
I mean, I would venture that if we were to remove alcohol
from university campuses, watch the students
are going to lobby against me if I say this,
but if you were to remove alcohol from campuses,
I mean, just think about the,
what I suspect would be the improvement
in good decision-making, and that would occur,
or, you know, I’ve got stories from graduate school,
and it was very different, you know, 10 years ago.
There was a lot more alcohol consumption.
Again, that was never my thing,
but I know people who make really bad decisions.
In any case, there’s a whole landscape there emerging.
I think you’ve got your finger right on the pulse of it.
I want to touch on something slightly different
than what we’ve been talking about,
but definitely related to depression,
and this, again, is one of these intriguing
but perplexing things, which is that sleep deprivation
can improve symptoms of depression,
and yet I’m personally very familiar with the fact
that if I don’t sleep well for one night,
or don’t sleep at all, in fact,
I do have an ability to function pretty well the next day.
I’ll do this non-sleep deep rest practice
that I blab a lot about on the Huberman Lab podcast,
which for me is tremendously restorative,
but I like a good night’s sleep.
I think everybody understands now,
thanks to the great work of Matthew Walker and others,
they’ve really gotten out into the world saying,
look, the foundation of mental health, physical health,
and high performance, if that’s your thing,
being a functional human being,
is to try and get enough quality deep sleep
at least 80% of the nights of your life, if you can.
That’s something to focus on, just like good nutrition,
just like exercise and social connection, et cetera.
So sleep deprivation, we know, can, in particular,
I think rapid eye movement components of sleep deprivation
can improve the symptoms of depression,
and yet being sleep deprived
can also really dysregulate our control
of the autonomic system.
I notice on night two or night three of poor sleep,
if I’m going through a stressful phase and that’s happening,
all of a sudden my heart rate is chronically elevated.
My thought patterns become really disrupted.
I can’t then exercise.
My decision-making is thrown off.
My emotionality is more labile.
The hinge, as we were referring to it earlier,
feels less in control, under my control,
and maybe I wonder sometimes if I enter that state
that you referred to earlier
where the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex
is no longer leading the cingulate,
but the cingulate is now in charge.
The players are in charge of the coach.
Not a good situation.
So I know you’ve done some work on sleep deprivation
and light and effects.
Please tell us about that,
and please tell us about this triple therapy.
Is that-
Yeah, yeah, so friend of mine, Greg Salem,
another one of the professors at Stanford,
was very interested in sleep.
He did a bunch of training in sleep
before he went to medical school
and got very interested in this idea that,
as you’re saying, if you sleep deprive somebody one night
in just kind of an isolated single night,
at the end of that sleep deprivation,
they will have an antidepressant effect,
but as soon as they fall asleep, they lose it.
So if it’s a depressed individual,
you can get them to be less depressed acutely
as soon as they fall asleep.
They wake up eight hours later,
then they come back into the same level of depression.
And so the idea was that you needed to do
some sort of circadian reset,
and that part of what depression is
is that it’s a dysregulated circadian system.
And so mentors of mine say,
if you can just get the sleep better,
that’s half the battle of dealing with depression
because so many people have insomnia around depression
and have a whole host of types of insomnia,
having a hard time falling asleep,
waking up in the middle of the night
and waking up earlier, all symptoms of depression.
And so what this does is it sleep deprives the individual
and then there’s a certain calculation
of shifting their phase
and simultaneously exposing them to bright light.
So that’s the triple, the phase shift,
the sleep deprivation and the bright light
to try to get their circadian rhythm.
Essentially, the theory is re-entrained.
And so in the trials that we’ve done
and other trials prior to ours and after,
it looked like there was a pretty profound
antidepressant effect from this triple therapy
that seemed to be durable,
meaning durability is this term we use
to say that not only can you get point relief,
but that the relief ends up lasting.
What’s important to know about this
is you shouldn’t do this at home for sure.
You would need to do this with a professional
because it’s complicated, it’s not just one thing.
And in sleep deprivation,
while it seems to be antidepressant, it’s pro-anxiety.
So if you take a highly anxious person
that’s not depressed and you sleep deprive them,
they get profoundly anxious.
And so that’s the other thing
that you have to really realize
is that this is like everything else
that I’ve talked about today,
all things that you have to do under medical supervision,
but curious, right?
And I think the question that always comes up
is why isn’t this used more?
And I think the reason is that
there’s not really a mechanism for,
ultimately in medicine, as sad as it is,
you have to have a code to do a thing.
There has to be a code associated with a treatment
and it’s hard to figure out how to make a code for this.
And so I think that’s part of it.
And so if there’s a way
and somebody’s got to kind of take that baton on that,
but if there’s a way to make a code for this,
I think you could actually turn it into something
that was more widely utilized.
And probably dream up ways of how to integrate AI,
passive sensing, all that stuff to really make that work.
But I think that would be the idea,
that would be the trajectory I’d see.
Yeah, having a billable to insurance code is fundamental.
And a lot of listeners to this podcast,
I think have a background in engineering science
and we will put a link to that manuscript
that talks about the triple therapy,
because here we’re talking about
one night sleep deprivation,
some time to light exposure to the eyes
and then shifting in the circadian clock,
things central to the themes of this podcast
that come up often.
I think for the typical person,
can we say that trying to get a regular light dark cycle
and sleep rhythm would be beneficial
for overall mood regulation?
Yeah, I think for the typical person,
really kind of re-regulating your sleep
and trying to get a good night’s sleep
in which you fall asleep, stay asleep,
wake up at a set time every morning
is going to be pretty crucial.
In mild depression, I think that one has a lot of control
over that as we were talking about earlier.
I think when you hit some threshold in depression
where things become kind of semi-volitional,
it’s harder to kind of will yourself into that.
There are therapies like,
there’s a CBT for insomnia, for instance,
where you can do cognitive behavioral therapy
to help with insomnia.
Sometimes people, and I’m no sleep expert,
I’d kind of pass this to Greg to fully talk about this,
but some of what goes on that people
with kind of milder insomnia experience
is like blue light out of their computer
and things like that that they,
so you can use like blue light blockers
and it tricks your brain, as you know better than me,
it tricks your brain to think that it’s still light outside
and so people will, they’ll have insomnia
because their brain still thinks that it’s light outside
and then people will, the kind of strict CBT for sleep,
therapists will say there are only two things
that you should do in your bed
and if you’re under a certain age and whatnot,
it’s really one thing that you should do in your bed,
which is to sleep and be with your partner, right?
And so those are kind of the two things
that you should do in a bedroom
and that’s really the only things
that you should do in a bedroom
if you’re having sleep problems.
You shouldn’t watch TV in a bedroom,
shouldn’t eat in a bedroom, shouldn’t hang out.
Keep the phone out of the bedroom.
Keep the phone out of the bedroom, yeah.
Yeah, we should get Greg Salem on the podcast.
I’ll just mention for people
that want to regulate their sleep,
we have a sleep toolkit that’s available
as a downloadable PDF at hubermanlab.com.
Just go to the menu
and a lot of the things in that toolkit
are based on work from Stanford Sleep Laboratories,
including Jamie Zeitzer’s and others’ lab,
not aimed at depression specifically.
Listen, Nolan, Dr. Williams,
this has been an amazing voyage
through the circuitry of autonomic control.
This landscape of the prefrontal cortex
is I find incredibly fascinating
and I just want to start off by saying,
please do come back again
and teach us more about that and your TMS work.
Before we wrap, however,
I do want to give you the opportunity
to talk about the SAINT study.
Is it SAINT or saints, plural?
Yeah, it’s SAINT.
So SAINT or what we’re calling it S-A-I-N-T now,
SAINT has, you know, the intent was not
to kind of connect it to religion,
but we may have accidentally done so.
We abbreviated it to S-A-I-N-T for the subsequent trials,
which was initially
Stanford Accelerated Intelligent Neuromodulation Therapy,
or now we’re calling Stanford Neuromodulation Therapy,
but the idea there, which is a cool idea,
is that TMS is a device that delivers a treatment
and the treatment is the protocol
and the protocol is the stimulation parameter set
in a specific brain region for a specific condition.
And so what’s cool about neuromodulation,
whether it be transcranial magnetic stimulation
or transcranial direct current stimulation
or deep brain stimulation,
like what Casey Halpern talked about on another podcast,
is this idea that in all of those cases,
the device itself is a physical layer conduit
of a stimulation protocol that’s therapeutic
for a given condition in a given brain region.
And so in the case of depression,
which we know the most about with TMS,
we’ve been doing TMS studies for depression since 1995,
right, and a clearance in 2008, 2009.
And in that timeframe, we were able to go
from really knowing very little at all
about how to do something like this
to getting an FDA clearance.
And the way that it went down was that
there were two groups studying different components at NIH.
The first group was studying mood neuroanatomy
on functional imaging that was kind of the first generation
of functional imaging back then, so PET scans,
which are kind of metabolic scans, and then SPECT scans.
And the idea there was looking at activity
and metabolism in prefrontal cortex.
And what they found in these kind of more crude scans
is a just general hypoactivity, hypometabolism.
The other group right upstairs at the National Institute
for Neurological Diseases and Stroke, NINDS,
they were looking at using TMS,
which had been around for 10 years,
and repetitively stimulating in motor cortex.
What they found was, gosh, we can get a readout
in thumb muscle movement amplitude
that’s really reproducible across people.
It’s universally reproducible.
And if we do certain stimulation approaches,
they are biologically active to either increase excitability,
i.e. the thumb motion on a set intensity goes up,
the amount of amplitude goes up,
or inhibitory, depotentiating,
it goes down with other biological stimulation approaches.
And then a third outcome, which is important,
that it’s inert, it doesn’t do either.
So you can have stimulation approaches that do one,
increase activity, decrease activity, or are inert.
And so what they found was,
oh, we can excite certain brain regions.
And my mentor, Mark George, had this kind of aha moment
where he said, wow, there’s underactivity
in prefrontal cortex in depression,
and we can increase activity using this thing
that we know we can increase activity in motor cortex,
we just need to put it
in the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex,
and then they combined the two and started stimulating
once a day in this kind of very abbreviated fashion,
and lo and behold, some of those depression patients
resolved their depression, and back then, and still today,
you can go, and as a psychiatric patient,
stay at the National Institute of Mental Health
and go through clinical trials to try to get treated,
and there were patients who’d been there for months,
and they were able to be discharged
because their mood was better.
Yeah, and so it was this very crude approach
where they were using ruler measurements
where DLPFC was, and they were stimulating with devices
that you needed to physically dunk the coil in an ice bath,
and with that, they still were able to,
the kind of genius of this, Mark and others,
they would still be able to create
a purely engineered stimulation approach.
What’s cool about that is that
they kind of found two things, right?
They found this one stimulation protocol
that does have some antidepressant effect.
It’s limited.
It doesn’t treat everybody.
It does have some antidepressant effect,
and this bigger concept that a neuromodulation device
is kind of like a pharmaceutical company for you, right,
that in a given individual, a TMS device
or whatever neuromodulation device is able to generate,
you can create a stimulation approach
that is specific to a given condition
and specific to an individual,
and so the physical layer is just how you exert that,
similarly to how we make pharmaceutical drugs
in a pharmaceutical company,
but the actual therapy itself is what you do
where you do it, and so what we learned from
another 20, 30 years of this
is that you can modify the stimulation protocol
in such a way where you can create a whole new treatment
and put it through the same TMS device,
or thank God, an evolved version of it
where you don’t have to dunk it in ice baths
and they can actually really handle
much more aggressive stimulation approaches,
and so in 2005, a group published in Neuron,
a paper demonstrating that if you stimulate
with the hippocampal rhythms through a TMS coil,
you can excite the brain with memory rhythms,
and it’ll last an hour,
so you can change cortical excitability
in this thumb twitch for an hour,
sending three minutes of excitatory,
or 40 seconds in the case of inhibitory stimulation
that mimics the hippocampal rhythms,
so much more efficient than the original TMS approaches,
and so after that, group tried to do it
in this kind of six-week schedule,
and after that, and while they were doing that,
we decided, gosh, this problem I talked about
at the beginning of the show where you have this problem
that we don’t have a treatment for people
who are in these high-acuity psychiatric emergency states,
this idea that we’re gonna engineer a treatment
where we can reorganize the stimulation approach in time
to be much more efficient by utilizing
something called space learning theory,
and so you probably know about the space learning theory,
so the idea for the viewers is
it’s a simple psychological thing,
but we’ve also seen it in hippocampal slice
sort of physiology too,
where if I’m cramming for a test,
what I do is I write out 60 note cards,
and I read each one for a minute
until I get to the first note card again,
and that’s about an hour later, right,
and we just intuitively do this.
We all automatically do that,
and we intuit that because we know
that what doesn’t work is writing out one note card
and looking at it over and over again.
Nobody ever does that, right?
We’ve all been in graduate school, medical school,
and we have these big stacks of note cards.
That’s space learning theory.
It’s this idea that you need to see it
about every hour to an hour and a half,
and that optimizes learning.
If you take the same stimulation approach
that I’m talking about,
this theta-burst stimulation approach,
and you take a hippocampal slice of a mouse
and you stimulate,
you enlarge some dendritic spines and you prime some,
and then if you stimulate right after that,
you don’t get any change.
It’s called in-mass stimulation,
but if you wait about an hour to an hour and a half,
you get more dendritic spines enlarged and more primed,
which, by the way, also is what ketamine does.
It causes this dendritic spine enlargement,
and so what we found was that the old way of doing TMS,
this idea of just doing it once a day,
every day, five days a week for six weeks,
didn’t utilize the space learning theory.
It’s like studying for a month or two,
just a little bit once a day.
You remember some of that stuff,
but it’s not as potent as that week
where you’re kind of cramming, right?
And what we realized is that if we could
reorganize the stimulation in time
so that we took the whole six-week course,
we actually figured out a way to do it in a day.
And then what we also figured out
is that people were underdosing TMS
because if you just keep going after six weeks
out to month three, four, five,
more and more people got better,
so we figured out it’s not just one day.
We’re gonna give five times the normal dose.
We have seven and a half months worth in five days
using space learning theory.
So every hour?
Every hour for 10 hours.
For five days.
For five days, so it’s a 50-hour block.
It’s 90 minutes of actual stimulation,
but spread out through the day in the same way of learning,
which is perfect for an inpatient psychiatric unit, right?
Five days is manageable.
Yeah, you can get stimulation.
Nobody’s ever dropped out by the schedule.
Folks that want to do this want to do it,
so they’ll do their nine minutes.
They’ll go get breakfast.
They’ll do their nine minutes.
They’ll go see their therapist or whatever it is,
and so what we found with this reorganization
in time of the stimulation, dose,
and then the third component is we do
resting state functional connectivity scans on everybody,
and we have ways now in the last five to 10 years
of picking out that specific subgenual DLPFC subcircuit
that I was talking about earlier, that cingulate DLPFC.
We can pick that out in every single one.
If you want to come to the lab,
we can find your DLPFC subgenual.
It’s even more robust than non-depressed.
Maybe we can stimulate too just while we’re in there.
Yeah, if you want to, we can move around
your hypnotizability, and we can find that spot
in each person, and instead of finding the same spot
on the skull, we find the same spot on the brain,
and we can stimulate, and we do that every hour on the hour,
and what we’ve found is that folks will,
within one to five days, in more cases than not,
depending upon if you’re looking at this open label
or in trials, somewhere between 60 and 90% of the time,
they will go into full-on remission in the sense
they’re totally normal from a mood standpoint
at the end of this, and like I said,
with variable durability, so that’s the part
we have to figure out now about dosing
and how to keep people well, but for some people,
we’ve had four years of remission, a year of remission,
and it’s really that cramming of the test.
It’s really that idea that you’re laying
in that information to the exact right spot,
and the signal’s a simple signal, but it’s a profound one,
which is turn on, stay on, remember to stay on.
You know, that idea that you’re sending this memory signal
into the brain, and you’re doing it in such a way
that you’re telling the system, you’re kind of taking it
out of the hippocampus’ hand, your own hippocampus’ hand,
and you’re sending the same signal
the hippocampus normally signals out.
Now you’re sending that signal
into the prefrontal cortex, and kind of utilizing
the brain’s own communication style
to get it to get out of the state,
and what’s very cool about this is that people,
when they kind of exit out of that,
they end up saying they don’t have any side effects from it,
and they feel back to normal.
Like some people, you know, not everybody,
but there’s a subsection of people with SSRIs
where they’ll say, I kind of feel numb,
or I have GI side effects, or I can’t, you know,
I don’t have the sexual interest that I used to have,
and that sort of thing.
You know, not anything against SSRIs,
as I said earlier, life-saving, you know,
for a subsection of people, these things really work,
but with this, what you see is that people
don’t talk about any of that stuff,
and I think it’s likely because you’re tapping
into that core circuitry, and you’re reversing it,
and you’re doing it with a magnet,
that because it’s a very profound electromagnet,
it’s the same field strength as an MRI scanner,
it’s able to induce a current in the brain
in this focal, targeted way,
without getting into the rest of the brain,
without getting into the rest of the body at all,
and just really kind of acting only
on that circuitry that’s involved.
Incredible.
Is the SAINT study still ongoing,
and if people are interested in potentially being
patients or subjects in the study,
can we provide them a portal link?
Absolutely, yeah.
So we have, now the treatment,
some of my students went over to a company
called Magnus Medical, and they’ve been working on this,
they’ve got an FDA clearance now,
and now folks can get it through trials
over the next couple of years,
because it’s going to take some time for that company
to kind of get up and running and get a device
and get the whole thing set up nationally,
but while that’s all going on,
there’s still about 1,000 patients
that need to be recruited across a bunch
of different trials all over the country.
We’ll take people from anywhere in the country.
We also have partners in New York and San Diego
and soon to be South Carolina and other places
where we can actually, my lab can help
to kind of let people know where to go,
based off of where they’re at in the US,
and get them access to being able to be in a trial,
and what we’ve tried to do is make it so that
even if you get the 50-50 chance
you’re going to get the real deal,
or you’re going to get the non-real deal,
but what we have figured out is a way
to let everyone have access.
If they got the not real deal version,
the kind of sham version or the fake version
for the first part of the trial,
there are other trials where they can have access
to the real version, so essentially everybody
eventually gets access to having the real version,
and so that’s been a big thing for me
is I want everybody that comes through one of our trials
to be able to have access.
I think it’s important while the company’s doing
what they’re doing and what the lab’s doing
and kind of nationally what other partner labs are doing.
Well, I can assure you, you’re going to get some interest.
Happy to have it, yeah.
Thank you.
And listen, thank you so much for taking us
on this incredible voyage through the neurocircuitry
underlying certain aspects of depression,
the coverage of the different types of depression,
the various therapeutic compounds, how they work.
We’ve talked about a lot of things today,
and you’ve shared so much knowledge,
and even as I say that, I very much want to have you back
to talk about many other things as well
that we didn’t have time to cover,
but also just really want to thank you
for the work that you do.
I know we are colleagues,
but you run an enormous laboratory,
enormous in my book, 40 people is a big group,
very big group, plus you’re in the clinic.
You also have a life of your own outside of work,
and to take the time to sit down with us
and share all this knowledge
that really is in service to mental health
and humans feeling better,
and in fact, avoiding often suicidal depression.
It’s just incredible work and an incredible generosity,
and just thank you so much.
Oh, thank you, man.
I mean, similarly, I want to thank you for what you’re doing.
I mean, I think that what you,
I’ve got a lot of friends,
folks that are not in the medical profession,
friends of mine, one of my buddies
who’s a real estate agent who works with us,
who’s a big fan of your show,
and I told a couple people like that I was coming on,
and they were super stoked.
They’re like, we watch every show,
and super excited to watch mine,
and they said something very important to me
that you make this complicated neuroscience
and brain-body science accessible
in a way that few have a gift to do,
and I think that that’s so important,
and this show is doing so much
to help with science literacy,
and yeah, appreciate you.
Well, thank you.
I’m gratified and honored by your statement,
and I look forward to more.
Thank you.
Absolutely, thank you.
Thank you for joining me today
for my discussion with Dr. Nolan Williams.
I hope you found our discussion
about psychedelics and other compounds,
about transcranial magnetic stimulation,
and about the treatments for depression in general
to be as stimulating as I did.
If you’d like to learn more
about the work being done in Dr. Williams’ laboratory,
you can go to the Brain Stimulation Laboratory website,
which is bsl.stanford.edu,
and there you have the opportunity to apply
to be in one of the clinical trials for depression
or other studies,
as well, if you’d like to support the work being done
in Dr. Williams’ laboratory
for the treatment of depression
and other psychiatric disorders.
If you’re learning from
and are enjoying the Huberman Lab Podcast,
please subscribe to our YouTube channel.
That’s a terrific zero-cost way to support us.
In addition, please subscribe to the Huberman Lab Podcast
on Spotify and Apple,
and on both Spotify and Apple,
you also have the opportunity
to leave us up to a five-star review.
If you have questions for us
or comments about the information we’ve covered
or suggestions about future guests,
please put those in the comment section on YouTube.
We do read all the comments.
Please also check out the sponsors mentioned
at the beginning of today’s episode.
That’s the best way to support the Huberman Lab Podcast.
Not so much today, but in many previous episodes
of the Huberman Lab Podcast, we talk about supplements.
While supplements aren’t necessary for everybody,
many people derive tremendous benefit from them
for things like enhancing sleep and focus
and hormone optimization.
The Huberman Lab Podcast
has partnered with Momentus Supplements.
If you’d like to see the supplements
that the Huberman Lab Podcast has partnered
with Momentus on, you can go to livemomentus,
spelled O-U-S, so livemomentus.com slash Huberman,
and there you’ll see a number of the supplements
that we talk about regularly on the podcast.
I should just mention that that catalog of supplements
is constantly being updated.
As mentioned at the beginning of today’s episode,
the Huberman Lab Podcast has now launched a premium channel.
That premium channel will feature monthly AMAs
or Ask Me Anythings where I answer your questions in depth,
as well as other premium resources.
If you’d like to subscribe to the premium channel,
you can simply go to hubermanlab.com slash premium.
I should mention that the proceeds from the premium channel
go to support the standard Huberman Lab Podcast,
which will continue to be released every Monday per usual,
as well as supporting various research projects
done on humans to create the sorts of tools
for mental health, physical health, and performance
that you hear about on the Huberman Lab Podcast.
Again, it’s hubermanlab.com slash premium to subscribe.
It’s $10 a month or $100 per year.
If you haven’t already subscribed
to our zero cost newsletter,
we have what is called the Neural Network Newsletter.
You can subscribe by going to hubermanlab.com,
go to the menu and click on newsletter.
Those newsletters include summaries of podcast episodes,
lists of tools from the Huberman Lab Podcast.
And if you’d like to see previous newsletters
that we’ve released,
you can also just go to hubermanlab.com,
click on newsletter in the menu,
and you’ll see various downloadable PDFs.
If you want to sign up for the newsletter,
we just ask for your email.
We do not share your email with anybody.
And again, it’s completely zero cost.
If you’re not already following me on social media,
it’s Huberman Lab on Twitter, on Facebook, and on Instagram.
And at all three of those places,
I cover topics and subject matter
that are sometimes overlapping with the information
covered on the Huberman Lab Podcast,
but that’s often distinct from information
on the Huberman Lab Podcast.
Again, it’s Huberman Lab on all social media channels.
And last, but certainly not least,
thank you for your interest in science.
♪ Yeah ♪