Huberman Lab - Dr. Eddie Chang: The Science of Learning & Speaking Languages

Welcome to the Huberman Lab Podcast,

where we discuss science

and science-based tools for everyday life.

I’m Andrew Huberman,

and I’m a professor of neurobiology and ophthalmology

at Stanford School of Medicine.

Today, my guest is Dr. Eddie Chang.

Dr. Eddie Chang is the chair of the neurosurgery department

at the University of California at San Francisco.

Dr. Chang’s clinical group focuses on the treatment

of movement disorders, including epilepsy.

He is also a world expert

in the treatment of speech disorders

and relieving paralysis that prevents speech

and other forms of movement and communication.

Indeed, his laboratory is credited with discovering ways

to allow people who have fully locked-in syndrome,

that is, who cannot speak or move,

to communicate through computers and AI devices

in order to be able to speak to others in their world

and understand what others are saying to them.

It is a truly remarkable achievement that we discuss today,

in addition to his discoveries about critical periods,

which are periods of time during one’s life

when one can learn things in particular languages

with great ease, as opposed to later in life.

And we talk about the basis of things like bilingualism

and trilingualism.

We talk about how the brain controls movement

of the very muscles that allow for speech and language

and how those can be modified over time.

We also talk about stutter,

and we talk about a number of aspects of speech

and language that give insight

into not just how we create this incredible thing

called speech or how we understand speech and language,

but how the brain works more generally.

Dr. Chang is also one of the world leaders

in bioengineering, that is the creation of devices

that allow the brain to function

at super physiological levels,

and that can allow people with various syndromes

and disorders to overcome their deficits.

So if you are somebody who is interested

in how the brain works normally,

how it breaks down and how it can be repaired,

and if you are interested in speech and language,

reading and comprehension of information of any kind,

today’s episode ought to include some information

of deep interest to you.

Dr. Chang is indeed the top of his field

in terms of understanding these issues

of how the brain encodes speech and language

and create speech and language,

and as I mentioned, movement disorders and epilepsy.

We even talk about things such as the ketogenic diet,

the future of companies like Neuralink,

which are interested in bioengineering

and augmenting the human brain, and much more.

One thing that I would like to note

is that in addition to being

a world-class neuroscience researcher

and world-class clinician neurosurgeon,

and chair of neurosurgery,

Dr. Eddie Chang has also been

a close personal friend of mine

since we were nine years old.

We attended elementary school together,

and we actually had a science club

when we were nine years old

focused on a very particular topic.

You’ll have to listen in to today’s episode

to discover what that topic was

and what membership to that club required.

That aside, Dr. Chang is an absolute phenom

with respect to his scientific prowess,

that is both his research and his clinical abilities,

and he’s one of these rare individuals

that whenever he opens his mouth, we learn.

Before we begin, I’d like to emphasize

that this podcast is separate

from my teaching and research roles at Stanford.

It is, however, part of my desire and effort

to bring zero cost to consumer information

about science and science-related tools

to the general public.

In keeping with that theme,

I’d like to thank the sponsors of today’s podcast.

Our first sponsor is Element.

Element is an electrolyte drink

with everything you need and nothing you don’t.

That means plenty of salt, magnesium, and potassium,

the so-called electrolytes, and no sugar.

Now, salt, magnesium, and potassium

are critical to the function of all the cells in your body,

in particular, to the function of your nerve cells,

also called neurons.

In fact, in order for your neurons to function properly,

all three electrolytes need to be present

in the proper ratios,

and we now know that even slight reductions

in electrolyte concentrations or dehydration of the body

can lead to deficits in cognitive and physical performance.

Element contains a science-backed electrolyte ratio

of 1,000 milligrams, that’s one gram, of sodium,

200 milligrams of potassium,

and 60 milligrams of magnesium.

I typically drink Element first thing in the morning

when I wake up in order to hydrate my body

and make sure I have enough electrolytes,

and while I do any kind of physical training

and after physical training as well,

especially if I’ve been sweating a lot.

If you’d like to try Element,

you can go to drinkelement, that’s lmnt.com slash Huberman

to claim a free Element sample pack with your purchase.

Again, that’s drinkelement, lmnt.com slash Huberman.

Today’s episode is also brought to us by Thesis.

Thesis makes what are called nootropics,

which means smart drugs.

Now, to be honest, I am not a fan of the term nootropics.

I don’t believe in smart drugs in the sense that

I don’t believe that there’s any one substance

or collection of substances that can make us smarter.

I do believe based on science, however,

that there are particular neural circuits

and brain functions that allow us to be more focused,

more alert, access creativity, be more motivated, et cetera.

That’s just the way that the brain works,

different neural circuits for different brain states.

Thesis understands this, and as far as I know,

they’re the first nootropics company

to create targeted nootropics for specific outcomes.

I’ve been using Thesis for more than six months now,

and I can confidently say that their nootropics

have been a total game changer.

My go-to formula is the clarity formula,

or sometimes I’ll use their energy formula before training.

To get your own personalized nootropic starter kit,

go online to takethesis.com slash Huberman,

take a three-minute quiz,

and Thesis will send you four different formulas

to try in your first month.

That’s takethesis.com slash Huberman,

and use the code Huberman at checkout

for 10% off your first order.

I’m pleased to announce that the Huberman Lab podcast

is now partnered with Momentus Supplements.

We partnered with Momentus for several important reasons.

First of all, they ship internationally

because we know that many of you are located

outside of the United States.

Second of all, and perhaps most important,

the quality of their supplements is second to none,

both in terms of purity and precision

of the amounts of the ingredients.

Third, we’ve really emphasized supplements

that are single ingredient supplements

and that are supplied in dosages

that allow you to build a supplementation protocol

that’s optimized for cost,

that’s optimized for effectiveness,

and that you can add things and remove things

from your protocol in a way

that’s really systematic and scientific.

If you’d like to see the supplements

that we partner with Momentus on,

you can go to livemomentus.com slash Huberman.

There you’ll see those supplements,

and just keep in mind that we are constantly expanding

the library of supplements available through Momentus

on a regular basis.

Again, that’s livemomentus.com slash Huberman.

And now for my discussion with Dr. Eddie Chang.

Eddie, welcome.

Hi, hi, Andrew.

Great to be here with you.

This has been a long time coming.

Just to come clean,

we’ve known each other since we were nine years old,

but then there was a long gap

in which we didn’t talk to one another.

I heard things about you,

and presumably you heard a thing or two about me,

for better or for worse.

And then we reconnected years later

when I was a PhD student and you were a medical student.

We literally ran into each other

in the halls of University of California, San Francisco,

where you’re now the chair of neurosurgery.

So it all comes full circle.

When you were at UCSF, you were working with Mike Merzenich,

and I know that name might not be familiar

to a lot of people,

but he’s sort of synonymous with neuroplasticity,

the ability of the brain and nervous system

to change in response to experience.

So for our listeners,

I would just love for you to give a brief overview

of what you were doing at that time,

because I find that work so fascinating,

and it really points to some of the things

that can promote and maybe hinder

our brain’s ability to change.

Oh, wow, that’s fantastic.

So we did bump into each other serendipitously back then.

And at the time I was a medical student at UCSF

studying with Mike Merzenich.

In particular, I was studying how the brain organizes

when you have patterns of sound.

And in particular, we were studying the brain of rodents

and trying to understand how different sound patterns

organize the frequency representation

from low to middle to high frequency maps

in the brains of baby rodents.

And one of the things that I was very interested in

was trying to understand

how the patterns of the natural environment,

let’s say the vocalizations of the environment

that the rat pups were raised in,

or just the natural sounds that they hear,

how that shapes the structure of the brain.

And one of the things we did was to try and experiment

where we raised some of these rat pups in white noise,

continuous white noise that was essentially masking

all of those environmental sounds.

And what was the consequence of animals

being raised in white noise environment?

Well, one of the things that we didn’t expect,

but we found which is quite striking

is that there’s this early period in brain development

where we’re very susceptible to the patterns

that we hear or see.

In neuroscience, we call this a critical period

or a sensitive period.

And we have this for our eyes,

but we also have it for our ears.

And one of the most striking examples of this

is that any human can essentially grow up in a culture

where they hear different speech sounds

from one language to another.

And it’s like after a couple of years,

you lose sensitivity to sounds

that are not part of your native language,

and you have high sensitivity

for the languages of your native culture.

And that’s pretty extraordinary

that human brain has that flexibility,

yet at the same time has that specialization for language.

And so we were trying to think about

how do we model this, for example, in rodents

who obviously don’t speak,

but we’re just understanding how sounds

and environmental sounds modulate

and organize the auditory cortex.

And one of the things that we found

that was quite striking was that

if you basically mask environmental sounds

from these rat pups, the critical period,

this sensitive period where it’s open to plasticity,

it’s open to change, it’s open to reorganization,

that actually, that window can stay open much, much longer.

And in one way, it sounds like that’s a good thing,

but on the other hand, it’s also a retardation.

It’s actually, it slowed the maturation

of the auditory cortex.

It was ready to close when these rat pups were really young,

but by raising them in white noise,

we found out that you could keep it open

for months beyond the time period that it normally closes.

And so I think one of the things it taught me

was that it’s not just about the genetic programming

that specifies some of this sensitive period,

but it’s also a little bit about the nature

of the sounds that we hear that help keep that window

for the critical period open and closed.

It’s fascinating.

And I know it’s difficult to make a direct leap

from animal research to human research,

but if we could speculate a little bit,

I can imagine that some people grow up in homes

where there’s a lot of shouting and a lot of inflection.

Maybe people are very verbose.

Maybe others grow up in a home

where it’s quieter and more peaceful.

Some people are going to grow up in cities.

I just came back from New York City.

It’s like all night long, there’s honking and sirens,

and it’s just nonstop.

And then I returned here where it’s quite quiet at night.

Can we imagine that the human brain

is going to be shaped differently

depending on whether or not

when it grows up in one environment or another?

And would that impact their tendency

to speak in a certain way,

as well as hear in a certain way?

What do we know about that?

Well, I think that it’s, from my perspective,

it’s really clear that those sounds that we are exposed to

from the very earliest time, even in utero, in the womb,

where the sound is hearing the mother or father or friends

around while in the womb,

actually will influence how these things organize.

And so there’s no question that the sounds that we hear

are going to have some influence.

And those sounds are going to structure

the way that those neural networks actually lay down

and will forever influence how you hear sounds.

And speech and language

is probably one of the most profound examples of that.

I get a lot of questions

about the use of white noise during sleep.

In particular, people want to know

whether or not using a white noise machine

or a machine or a program

that makes the sound of waves, for instance,

if it assists their infant in sleeping,

is it going to be bad for them

because it’s flooding the auditory system

with a bunch of essentially white noise

or disorganized noise?

Do we have an answer to that question?

Not yet.

I think that what you’re asking

is a really important question

because parents are using white noise generators

almost universally now.

And for good reasons, you know,

it is hard to have kids up at night.

I’ve got three kids of my own

and was very tempted to think about

how to use some of these tools

to just soothe them and get them to bed,

especially when I was like so tired and exhausted.

But I think that there is a cost, you know,

to think a little bit about, you know,

we’re not exposed to continuous white noise naturally.

There is a value to having really salient structured sounds

that are part of our natural environment

to actually have the brain develop normally.

So whether or not that has an impact, you know,

while you’re sleeping, it’s not clear.

I don’t think that those studies have been done.

What was really clear was that

if you raise these baby rats in continuous white noise,

not super loud, but just enough

to mask the environmental sounds,

that that was enough to keep, you know,

the auditory cortex, the part of the brain that hears

in this really delayed state,

which could essentially slow down the development

and maturation of the brain.

And one could probably assume that

slowing the maturation of areas of the brain

they’re responsible for hearing might,

I want to underscore,

might impact one’s ability to speak, right?

Because isn’t it the case that if people can’t hear,

they actually have a harder time enunciating

in a particular way?

Is that right?

If I were to not be able to hear my own voice,

would my speech patterns change?

Well, I think part of it is that over time,

we develop sensitivity to the very specific speech sounds

in a given language.

And the sensitivity improves

as we hear more and more and more of it.

And then on the other hand,

we lose sensitivity to other speech sounds at the same time.

But as part of that process,

we also have a selectivity,

again, a specialization even for those sounds.

Even relative to noise,

noisy backgrounds and things like that.

I tend to think about it like

what is the signal to noise ratio?

And so the brain has its own ways

of trying to increase that signal to noise ratio

in order to make it more clear.

Part of that is how we hear

and how it lays down a foundation

for that signal to noise ratio.

And so you can imagine a child

that’s raised continuously in white noise

would be really deprived of those kinds of sounds

that are really necessary for it to develop properly.

So I think with regard to those tools for babies,

I think we should study,

we should try to understand this definitively.

I think what we saw in rodents

would tell us that there is potential,

things that we should be concerned about.

But again, it’s not really clear

if you’re just using at night,

whether it has those effects.

I guess the critical question

that a number of people are going to be asking is,

did you decide to use a white noise machine

or not to help keep any of your three children asleep?

Well, I think the short answer is no.

I mean, I obviously did a lot of work thinking

and work on this and thought about it carefully,

but there are other kinds of noise,

or I wouldn’t even call it noise,

other sounds that you can use

that can be equally soothing to a baby.

It’s just that white noise has no structure

and what it’s doing is essentially masking out

all of the natural sounds.

And I think the goal should really be about

how do we replace that with other more natural sounds

that structure the brain in the way

that we want to be more healthy.

Well, I know that after you finished your medical training,

you went on to, of course, specialize in neurosurgery.

And last I checked, you spend most of your days

either running your laboratory or in the clinic

or running the department.

And your clinical work and your laboratory work

involves often removing pieces of the skull of humans

and going in and either removing things

or stimulating neurons,

treating various ailments of different kinds.

But your main focus these days, of course,

is the neurobiology of speech and language.

And so for those that aren’t familiar,

could you please distinguish for us speech versus language

in terms of whether or not different brain areas

control them?

And I know that there’s a lot of interest

in how speech and language and hearing

all relate to one another.

And then we’ll talk a bit about, for instance,

emotions and how facial expressions could play into this

or hand gestures, et cetera.

But for the uninformed person

and for me to be quite direct,

what are the brain areas that control speech and language?

What are they really, and especially in humans,

how are they different?

I mean, we have such sophisticated language

compared to a number of other species.

What does all this landscape look like in there?

Yeah, well, that’s a fascinating question.

And I’m going to just try to connect

a couple of the dots here,

which is that in that earlier work during medical school,

I was doing a lot of what we call neurophysiology,

putting electrodes into the auditory cortex

and understanding how the brain responds to sounds.

And that’s how we actually mapped out these things

about the sensitivity to sensitive periods.

That experience with Mike Merzenich

and thinking about how plasticity is regulating the brain,

in particular about how sound is represented

by brain activity, was something that

was really formative for me.

And because I was a medical student

and I was going back to my medical studies,

it was that in combination with

seeing some awake brain surgeries

that our department is really well known for.

One of my mentors, Mitch Berger,

really pioneered these methods

for taking care of patients with brain tumor

and be able to do these surgeries safely

by keeping patients awake and by mapping out language.

So they’re talking and listening in your head,

essentially in conversation with these patients

while there’s a portion of their skull removed

and you are stimulating or in some cases

removing areas of their brain.

Is that right?

That’s exactly right.

And the only thing off there is it’s not essentially,

it is just that.

The only difference between the conversation

that I might have with my patient

who’s undergoing awake brain surgery

is that I can’t see their face and they can’t see my face.

We actually have a sterile drape

that actually separates the operating field

and they’re looking and interacting

with our neuropsychologists.

But I can talk to them and they can hear my voice

and vice versa.

And it’s a really, really important way

of how we can protect some of those areas

that are really critical for language.

At the same time, accomplish the mission

of getting the seizures under control

or getting a brain tumor removed.

And is that because occasionally

you’ll encounter a brain area,

maybe you’re stimulating,

you’re considering removing that brain area

and suddenly a patient will start stuttering

or will have a hard time formulating a sentence.

Is that essentially what you’re looking for?

You’re looking for regions in which it is okay

or not okay to probe?

Exactly.

So the first thing that we do

is that we use a small electrical stimulator

to probe different parts of the areas

that we think might be related and important for language

or talking or even movements of your arm and leg.

That’s what we call brain mapping.

And we use a small electrical current

that’s delivered through a probe

that we can just put at each spot.

And the areas that we’re really interested in

are of course the areas that are right around

the part that is pathological,

the part that’s injured or the part that has a brain tumor

that we want to remove.

So we can apply that probe and transiently,

meaning temporarily, activate it.

So if you’re stimulating the part of the brain

that controls the hand, the hand will move.

It will jerk.

Sometimes a fist will be made, something like that.

Other times, while someone is counting

or just saying the days of the week,

you can stimulate in a different area

that stops their speech altogether.

That’s what we call speech arrest.

Or if someone is looking at pictures

and they’re describing the pictures

and you’re stimulating a particular area,

they stop speaking or the words start coming out slurred

or they can’t remember the name of the object

that they’re seeing in the picture.

These are all things that we’re listening really carefully

while we apply that focal stimulation.

That’s what we call brain mapping.

What are some of the more surprising,

or maybe even if you want to offer

one of the more outrageous examples

of things that people have suddenly done

or failed to be able to do

as a consequence of this brain mapping?

Well, I think the thing to me

that has been the most striking

is that some of these areas you stimulate

and altogether you can shut down someone’s talking.

So a person says,

I wanted to say it, but I couldn’t get the words out.

And even though I’ve seen this thousands of times now,

it’s still exciting every time that I see it

because it’s exciting because you’re seeing the brain.

It’s a physical organ.

It’s part of the body.

Outside of the veins, on top of it,

doesn’t look like a machine.

But when you do something like that

and you focally change the way it works,

and you see that because a person can’t talk anymore,

and they say, I know what I want to say,

but I couldn’t get the words out,

you’re confronted with this idea

that that organ is the basis of speech and language,

and way beyond that, obviously,

for all the other functions that we have for thinking

and feeling our emotions, everything.

So that to me is a constant reminder

of this really special thing that the brain does

was compute so many of the things that we do,

and in particular in the area around speech and language,

generating words,

something that is really unique to our species

is just extraordinary to see.

Again, even though I’ve seen it thousands of times,

it’s just having that connection

because it doesn’t look like machine,

but it is doing something

that is quite complicated, precise, and remarkable.

Do you ever see emotional responses

from stimulation in particular areas?

And do you ever hear or see emotional responses

that are associated with particular types of speech?

Because for instance, curse words are known to,

people with Tourette’s often will curse, not always,

but sometimes they’ll have tics or other things.

But what I learned from a colleague of ours

is that curse words have a certain structure to them.

There’s usually a heavy

or kind of a sharp consonant upfront, right?

That allows people, at least as it was described to me,

to have some sort of emotional release.

It’s not a word like murmur,

which has a kind of a soft entry here.

I’m not using the technical language.

And you pick your favorite curse word out there, folks.

I’m not going to shout out any now or say any now,

but that certain words have a structure to them

that because of the motor patterns

that are involved in saying that word,

you could imagine has an emotional response unto itself.

So when stimulating

or when blocking these different brain areas,

do you ever see people get angry or sad or happy

or more relaxed?

Oh, well, definitely I’ve seen cases

where you can invoke anxiety, stress.

And I think that there are also areas

that you can stimulate and you can also evoke

the opposite of that, sort of like a calm state.

I think that brain area is slightly hyperactive in you,

or at least more than me.

And all the years I’ve known you,

you’ve always been, at least externally, a very calm person.

I mean, I always find it amusing

that you work on speech and language

and you have a very calming voice, right?

And I’m being really serious.

I think that there is a huge variation there, right?

In terms of how people speak and how they accent words.

Absolutely, yeah.

So there are areas, for example,

the orbital frontal cortex that we showed,

that if you stimulate there,

the orbital frontal cortex is a part of the brain

that’s above the eyes.

That’s why they call it orbital frontal,

meaning it’s above the eye or the orbit

and in the frontal lobe.

And it’s this area right in here.

It has really complex functions.

It’s really important for learning and memory.

But one of the things that we observed

is when you stimulate there,

people tended to have a reduction in their stress.

And it was very much related to their state of being,

meaning that if someone was already kind of feeling normal

and you stimulate there, it didn’t do much.

But if someone was in a very anxious state,

it actually relieved that.

And then we’ve seen the corollary of that,

which is true too,

which is that there are other areas like the amygdala

or parts of the insula that if you stimulate,

you can cause an acute, temporary anxiety,

a nervous feeling.

Or if you stimulate the insula,

people can have an acute feeling of disgust.

So, you know, the brain has different functions

and these different nodes

that help process the way we feel.

Certainly, I think that to some degree,

neuropsychiatric conditions reflect an imbalance

of the electrical activities in these areas.

One of the things that was something I will never forget

was taking care of a young woman with uncontrolled seizures.

We call that epilepsy.

It’s a medical condition

where someone has uncontrolled electrical activity

in the brain.

Sometimes you can see that as convulsions

where people are shaking and lose consciousness.

There are other kinds of seizures that people can have

where they don’t lose consciousness,

but they can have experiences that just come out of nowhere.

And it’s just as a result of electrical activity

coming from the brain.

And about six years ago, I took care of a young woman

who was diagnosed psychiatrically with anxiety disorder

for several years.

It turns out that it wasn’t really an anxiety disorder.

It was actually that she had underlying seizures

and epilepsy activating a part of her brain

that evokes, you know, anxious feelings.

How did, how was that discovered?

Because I know a lot of people out there have anxiety.

I mean, in the absence of a brain scan,

how would, or why would one suspect

that maybe they have a tumor or some other condition

that was causing those neurons to become hyperactive?

Yeah, that’s really important

because so many people have anxiety

and the vast, vast majority are not having that

because they’re having seizures in the brain.

I think one of the ways that this was diagnosed

was that the nature of when she was having

these panic attacks was not triggered by anything.

They would just happen spontaneously.

And that’s what can happen with seizures sometimes.

They just come out of nowhere.

We don’t fully understand what can trigger them,

but they weren’t things

that were typically anxiety provoking.

This is something that just happened all of a sudden.

And because you brought it up,

this is not something that you can see on an MRI.

We could not see and look at the structure of her brain

with an MRI that she was having seizures.

The only way that we could actually prove this

was actually putting electrodes into her brain

and proving that these attacks that she was having

were localized to a part called the amygdala.

It’s a medial part of the temporal lobe, which is here.

And associating the electrical activity

that we’re seeing on those electrodes

with the symptoms that she had.

And she ultimately needed a kind of surgery

where she was awake in order to remove this safely.

Speaking of epilepsy,

a number of people out there have epilepsy

or know people who do.

Are the drugs for epilepsy satisfactory?

I think about things like Depakote

and adjusting the excitation and inhibition of the brain.

Are there good drugs for epilepsy?

We know there are not great drugs

for a lot of other conditions.

But, and how often does one need neurosurgery

in order to treat epilepsy?

Or can it be treated most often just using pharmacology?

Yeah, great question.

Well, a lot of people have seizures

that can be completely controlled by their medications.

A lot.

But there’s about a one third of people who have epilepsy,

which we define as anyone who’s had three or more seizures,

that about a third of them actually don’t have control

with all of the modern medications that we have nowadays.

And some of the data suggests

that if you have two or three medications,

it actually doesn’t matter necessarily

which of the anti-seizure medications it is.

But there’s data suggests if you’ve just tried two or three,

the fourth, fifth, sixth, and beyond

is not likely to help control it.

So we are in a situation, unfortunately,

where a lot of the medications are great for some people,

but for another subset, they can’t control it.

And it comes from a particular part of the brain.

Now, fortunately, in that subset,

there’s another part of that group

that can benefit from a surgery

that actually either removes that part of the brain,

and nowadays we’ll use stimulators now

to sometimes put electrical stimulation

in that part of the brain to help reduce the seizures.

And you said a third of people with epilepsy

might need neurosurgery.

Well, what I mean by that is like,

they continue to have seizures

that are not controlled by all medications.

And there’s going to be another subset of those

that may benefit from a surgery.

It’s probably not that whole third.

It’s a subset of that.

It’s just to say that epilepsy

can be really hard to get fixed.

And for people where the seizures come from one spot

or an area, then surgery can do great.

If it comes from multiple areas

or if it comes from the whole brain,

then we have to think about other methods to control it.

Fortunately, nowadays, there’s actually other ways.

Surgery now to us doesn’t just mean

removing part of the brain.

Half of what we do now is use stimulators

that modulate the state of the brain

that can help reduce the seizures.

I’ve heard before that the ketogenic diet

was originally formulated in order to treat epilepsy,

and in particular in kids.

Is that true?

And why would being in a ketogenic state

with low blood glucose reduce seizures?

That’s a great question.

And to be honest, I don’t know actually

if it was originally designed to treat seizures,

but I can tell you for sure

that for some people, just like with some medications,

it can be a life-changing thing.

It can completely change the way that the brain works.

And it’s not something that’s for everybody,

but for some people, there’s no question

that it has some very beneficial effects.

I think it’s to be determined still,

like why and how that works.

I’ve heard similar things about the ketogenic diet

for people with Alzheimer’s, dementia,

that there’s nothing particularly relevant

about ketosis to Alzheimer’s per se,

but because Alzheimer’s changes the way

that neurons metabolize energy,

that shifting to an alternate fuel source

can sometimes make people feel better.

And so a number of people are now trying it,

but it’s not as if blood glucose

and having carbohydrates is causing Alzheimer’s.

And people get confused often

that just because something can help

doesn’t mean that the opposite is harming somebody.

So I find this really interesting.

Sometime I’ll check back with you

about what’s happening in terms of ketogenic diets

and epilepsy, but you said that in some cases it can help.

Has that observation been made

both for children and for adults?

Because I thought that originally the ketogenic diet

for epilepsy was really for pediatric epilepsy.

Yeah, that’s right.

So a lot of its focus has really been on kids with epilepsy,

but certainly it’s a safe thing to try.

So a lot of adults will try it as well.

Interesting.

I’d like to take a quick break

and acknowledge one of our sponsors, Athletic Greens.

Athletic Greens, now called AG1,

is a vitamin mineral probiotic drink

that covers all of your foundational nutritional needs.

I’ve been taking Athletic Greens since 2012,

so I’m delighted that they’re sponsoring the podcast.

The reason I started taking Athletic Greens

and the reason I still take Athletic Greens

once or usually twice a day

is that it gets me the probiotics

that I need for gut health.

Our gut is very important.

It’s populated by gut microbiota

that communicate with the brain, the immune system,

and basically all the biological systems of our body

to strongly impact our immediate and long-term health.

And those probiotics in Athletic Greens

are optimal and vital for microbiotic health.

In addition, Athletic Greens contains a number of adaptogens,

vitamins, and minerals that make sure

that all of my foundational nutritional needs are met,

and it tastes great.

If you’d like to try Athletic Greens,

you can go to athleticgreens.com slash Huberman,

and they’ll give you five free travel packs

that make it really easy to mix up Athletic Greens

while you’re on the road, in the car, on the plane, et cetera,

and they’ll give you a year’s supply of vitamin D3K2.

Again, that’s athleticgreens.com slash Huberman

to get the five free travel packs

and the year’s supply of vitamin D3K2.

I’m curious about epilepsy for another reason.

I was taught that epilepsy is an imbalance

in the excitation and inhibition in the brain.

So you think about these electrical storms

that give people either grand mal,

you know, shaking and kind of convulsions.

But years ago, I was reading a book,

a wonderful book, actually,

called The Einstein in Love by Dennis Overby.

It was about Einstein and his more,

I guess, his personal life.

But people who knew him claimed

that he would sometimes walk along

and then every once in a while would just stop

and kind of stare off into space

for anywhere from a minute to three or five minutes.

And it was speculated that he had absence seizures.

What is an absence seizure?

And the reason I ask is I occasionally

will be walking along and I’ll be thinking

about something and I’ll stop.

But in my mind, I think I’m thinking during that time.

But I realized that if I were to see myself

from the outside, it might appear

that I was just kind of absent.

What is an absence seizure?

Because it’s so strikingly different in its description

from, say, a grand mal convulsive seizure.

Sure.

Well, like I mentioned before,

depending on how the seizure activity spreads

in the brain or how it actually propagates,

if it stays in one particular spot

and doesn’t spread to the entire brain,

it can have really different manifestation.

It can represent really differently.

So absence seizure is just one category

of different kinds of seizures

where you can lose consciousness, basically.

And what I mean by that is that you’re not fully aware

of what’s going on in your environment.

Okay, so you’re sort of taken offline temporarily

from consciousness, but you could still be,

for example, standing.

And to people who are not paying attention,

they may not even be aware that that’s happening.

What are some other types of seizures?

Well, I think some of the other kinds are,

the classic ones are temporal lobe seizures.

So these are ones that come from the medial structures

like the amygdala and hippocampus.

Oftentimes people, when they have seizures coming from that,

they may taste something very unusual,

like a metallic taste or smell,

something like the smell of burning toast,

something like that.

There are some people with temporal lobe seizures

will have deja vu.

They will have that experience

that you’ve been somewhere before,

but that’s just a precursor to the seizure.

And it just highlights that when people have seizures

coming from these areas,

they sometimes hijack what that part of the brain

is really for.

So the amygdala and hippocampus, for example,

are really important for learning and memory.

It’s not surprising that when people have seizures there,

that it can invoke a feeling of deja vu

or that it can invoke a feeling of anxiety.

And in the areas that are right next to it, for example,

these areas are really important for processing smell.

So these areas are right next to each other.

So you can have these kind of complex set of symptoms,

the weird taste, the smell of toast,

and then a feeling of deja vu.

That’s classic for temporal lobe seizure.

And it’s because those parts of the brain

that process those functions are right next to each other.

I’m told that I’ve had nocturnal seizures

and I’ve woken up sometimes from sleep

having felt as if I was having a convulsion,

a sort of sense of buzzing in the back of the head.

This happened to me two or three times in college.

My girlfriend, well, I woke up

and my girlfriend was very distraught.

Like you were having a seizure.

I was having full convulsion in my sleep.

What are, is that correct?

Are there, is there such a thing as nocturnal seizures?

What do they reflect?

They eventually stopped happening

and I couldn’t tether them to any kind of life event.

I wasn’t doing any kind of combat sport

or anything at the time.

I wasn’t drinking alcohol much.

It’s never really been my thing.

What are nocturnal seizures about?

Oh, well.

And do I need brain surgery?

Nocturnal seizures are just another form.

Like again, epilepsy and seizures

can have so many different forms

and not just like where in the brain,

but also when they happen.

And there are some people who for whatever reason,

it’s very timed to the circadian rhythm.

There’s actually not just happening at night,

but a certain period at night

when people are in a certain stage of sleep

that the brain is in a state

that it’s vulnerable to having a seizure.

And so that’s basically just one form of that.

Again, it’s not just about where it’s coming from,

but also when it’s happening and how that’s timed

with other things that are happening with the body.

Interesting.

Well, it eventually stopped happening.

So I stopped worrying about it,

but I haven’t had seizures since.

Returning to speech and language.

When I was getting weaned in neuroscience,

I learned that we have an area of the brain

for producing speech,

and we have an area of the brain for comprehending speech.

What’s the story there?

Is it still true that we have a Broca’s and a Wernicke’s area?

Those are names of neurologists presumably,

or neurosurgeons that discover these different brain areas.

Maybe you could familiarize us

with some of the sort of textbook version

of how speech and language are organized in the brain.

Maybe share with us a little bit of the lesion studies

that led to that understanding.

And then I would love to hear a bit

about what your laboratory is discovering

about how things are actually organized,

because from some discussions you and I have had

over the last year or so, it seems like,

well, let’s just be blunt.

It seems that much of what we know

from the textbooks could be wrong.

Well, I love that question,

because for me, it’s very central to the research we do,

and it’s where the intersection

between what we do in the laboratory

and our research interfaces with what I see in patients.

And one of the things that fascinated me early on

in my medical training was,

in doing some of these brain mapping

or watching them with my mentor,

or taking care of patients that had brain tumors

in a certain part of the brain,

was that a lot of times what I was seeing in a patient

did not correlate with what I was taught in medical school.

And some people will think,

well, this might be an exception.

But after you see it for a couple of times,

and if you’re kind of interested in this problem,

it poses a serious challenge to what you’ve learned

and how you think about how these things operate.

And that actually got me really interested

in trying to figure this out,

because earlier we talked about just this extraordinary thing

that the brain is doing to create words and sentences.

And that’s the process by which I’m getting ideas out

from my mind into yours.

It’s an incredible thing, right?

It’s the basis of communication,

high information communication between two individuals

that’s really unique to humans.

So in historical times,

how this works has been very controversial

from day one of neuroscience.

A long time ago, people thought the bumps on your head

corresponded to the different faculties of the mind.

So for example, if you had a bump here,

it might be corresponding to intelligence,

or another one over here, to vision

and these kinds of things.

That’s what we nowadays call phrenology.

And that was kind of the starting point.

A lot of that has been, of course, debunked.

But when you see those little statues

of different brain partitions on someone’s head,

that’s essentially how people were thinking

about how the brain worked back then,

couple of hundred years ago.

Modern neuroscience began when,

actually was very much related

to the discovery of language.

So modern neuroscience, meaning moving beyond this idea

that the bumps on the scalp

corresponded to the faculties of the mind.

But there were things that actually

were in the brain themselves,

and they weren’t corresponding to things

that you could see superficially,

like on the scalp or externally.

That it was something about the brain itself.

I mean, it seems so obvious now,

but back then this was the big academic debate.

And the first observation

that I think really was really impactful

in the area of language was an observation

by a neurosurgeon, a French neurosurgeon named Pierre Broca.

And what he observed was that in a patient,

not that he did surgery,

but that he had seen and taken care of,

that the person couldn’t talk.

And in particular, they called this individual tan,

because the only words that he could produce was tan, tan.

For the most part, he could generally understand

the kind of things that people were asking him about,

but the only thing that he could utter from his mouth

were these words tan, tan.

And what eventually had happened

was this individual passed away.

And the way that neuroscience was done back then

was basically to wait until that happened

and then to remove the brain

and to see what part of the brain was affected

in this patient that they called tan.

And what Broca found was that there was a part

in the left frontal lobe.

So the frontal lobe is this area like I described earlier,

which is behind our forehead up here.

And in the back of that frontal lobe,

he claimed that this was the seat of articulation

in the brain.

He literally used something like that in French,

the seat of articulation,

meaning that this is the part of the brain

that is responsible for us to generate words.

About 50 years later, the story becomes more complicated

with a German neurologist named Karl Wernicke.

And what Wernicke described was a different set of symptoms

in patients that he observed a different phenomenon

where people could produce words,

but a lot of the word,

and they were fluent in the sense that they had like,

they sound like they could be real words,

but from a different language, for example.

And some of us call that like word salad or jargon.

It’s essentially, they were essentially making up words,

but it was not intentional.

It was just the way that the words came out.

But in addition to that,

he observed that these people also could not understand

what was being said to them.

So we could be having conversation

and I’d be asking you, am I a woman?

And you might nod your head,

just because you’re not processing the question.

And so here are two observations.

One is that the frontal lobe is important

for articulating speech,

creating the words and expressing them fluently.

And then a different part of the brain

called the left temporal lobe,

which is this area right above my ear.

That is an area that I think was claimed

to be really important for understanding.

So the two major functions in language,

to speak and to understand,

were kind of pinned down to that.

And we’ve had that basic idea in the textbooks

for over 200 years.

Certainly what I was taught.

Is that right?

Oh, every, yeah.

And certainly what we still,

we still teach undergraduates,

graduate students and medical students that.

Well, that’s what I learned too in medical school.

And what I saw in reality

when I started taking care of patients

was that it’s not so simple.

In fact, part of it is fundamentally wrong.

So just in a nutshell,

nowadays, after looking at this very carefully

over hundreds of patients,

we’ve shown that surgeries, for example,

in the posterior part of the frontal lobe,

a lot of times people have no problem talking at all,

whatsoever after those kinds of surgeries.

And that it’s a different part of the brain,

that we call the precentral gyrus.

The precentral gyrus is a part of the brain

that is intimately associated with the motor cortex.

The motor cortex is the part of the brain

that has a map of your entire body.

So that has a part that corresponds to your feet,

has a part that corresponds to your hands.

But then there’s another part

that comes out more laterally on the side of the brain

that corresponds to your lips, your jaw, your larynx.

And we have seen that when patients have surgeries

or injuries to that part of the brain,

it actually can really interrupt language.

So it’s not as simple as just moving

the muscles of the vocal tract,

but it’s also important for formulating

and expressing words.

So that’s Broca’s area

that I think the field now recognizes,

not just because of our work,

but many other people that have studied this

in stroke and beyond,

is that the idea that that is the basis

of speaking in Broca’s area

is fundamentally wrong right now.

And we have to figure out how to correct the textbooks

that we kind of understand that

so that we can continue to make progress.

Now, in terms of the other major area

that we call Wernicke’s area,

the posterior temporal lobe,

that has held,

held, I think, quite legitimately for some time.

So that is an area that you have to be super careful

when you do surgery there.

That’s an area where if you have a mistake there

and you cause a stroke

or you remove too much of the tumor there,

you go too far beyond it,

then the person can be really, really hurt.

Like they’ll have a condition that we call aphasia,

where they may not be able to understand words.

They may not be able to remember the word

that they’re trying to say.

They know what they’re trying to say,

but they can’t remember the precise word

that goes with the object that they’re trying to think of.

They may even produce words that I described before

are like word salad or very jargony.

So, you know, they might say something like,

Tamir and I.

That’s not a real word,

but it sounds like it could be, you know?

And that’s just because that part of the brain

has some role, not just in understanding what we hear,

but also actually has a really important role

in sending the commands to different parts of the brain

to control what we say.

Not long ago, you and me and my good friend, Rick Rubin,

were having a conversation about medicine and science.

And Rick asked the question,

what percentage of what you learned in graduate

and or medical school do you think is correct?

And you had a very interesting answer.

Would you share it with us?

I don’t know.

I don’t remember the exact,

but I would say that with regard to the brain in particular,

I would say about 50% gets it right and accurate

and is helpful.

But another 50% is just the approximation

and oversimplification of what’s going on.

The example that we talked about language,

just an example of that.

It’s just, there are things that make it easier to learn

and easier to teach and easier to even think about.

And that’s probably why we continue teaching

in the way that we do.

But I think as time goes on,

the complexity of reality of how the brain works

is, well, first of all, we’re still trying to figure it out.

And second of all, it is complex

and it’s still incomplete story.

It’s early days.

And we get into some of the technical advances

that are allowing some correction of the errors

that the field has made.

And look, no disrespect to the brain explorers

that came before us.

And the ones that come after us will correct us, right?

That’s the way the game is played.

But what I’m hearing is that there are certain truths

that people accept.

And then there’s about half of the information

that is still open for debate

and maybe even for complete revision.

One thing that I learned about language

and the neural circuits underlying language

is that it’s heavily lateralized.

That these structures, Broca’s and Wernicke’s

and other structures in the brain

responsible for speech and comprehension of speech

sit mainly on one side of the brain,

but they do not have a mirror representation

or another equivalent area

on the opposite side of the brain.

And for those that haven’t poked around in a lot of brains,

certainly you Eddie have done far more of that than I have,

but I’ve done my fair share in non-human species

and a little bit in humans.

Almost every structure, almost every structure

has a matching structure on the other side of the brain.

So when we say the hippocampus,

we really mean two hippocampi,

one on each side of the brain.

But language I was taught is heavily lateralized.

That is that there’s only one.

So that raises two questions.

One, is that true?

And if it is true,

then what is the equivalent real estate

on the opposite side of the brain doing?

If it’s not doing the same function

that the one on the, say the left side is performing?

Well, that’s one of those things that is again,

like mostly true, not a hundred percent.

And what I mean by that is that it’s complicated.

So for people who are right-handed,

99% of the time,

the language part of the brain is on the left side.

And what is the equivalent brain area

on the right side doing if it’s not doing language?

Well, you know, the thing that’s incredible

is if you look at the right side

and you look at it very carefully,

either under an MRI,

or you actually look at the brain

under slides at a microscope,

it looks very, very similar.

It’s not identical, but it looks very, very similar.

All the gyri, which are the bumps on the brain that,

you know, have the different contours

and the valleys that we call soul side,

those all look basically the same.

Like there is a mirror anatomy on the left and right side.

And so it’s not been so clear what’s so special actually

about the left side to house language.

But what we do know,

and this is what we use all the time in assessing

and figuring out, you know, this before surgery,

is if you’re right-handed,

99% of the time the language is going to be

on the left side of the brain.

Is handedness genetic in any way?

I mean, when I grew up,

a pen or pencil or crayon was placed into my hand,

presumably, or I started using it.

My father was left-handed

and then where he grew up in South America,

they forced him to force himself to become right-handed.

They actually used to restrict the movement

of his left hand, so he was forced to right.

So, and then you have hook lefties and hook righties.

I know this is a deep dive

and we probably don’t want to go

into every derivation of this.

So for somebody who’s left-handed,

naturally just starts writing with the left hand,

there’s some genetic predisposition to being left-handed?

Absolutely, no question about it.

Handedness is not entirely, but strongly genetic.

So there is something about that ties all of this.

And what does handedness, for example,

have to do with where the part of your brain

that controls language?

Well, it turns out that the parts that control the hand

are very close to the areas that really are responsible

for the vocal tract.

Again, part of the motor cortex

and part of this brain area called the precentral gyrus.

And there are some theories that because of their proximity,

that these parts of the brain might develop together early

in utero and they might have a headstart

compared to the right side.

And because they have a headstart,

that things solidify there.

This is one theory of why this happens.

And people who are left-handed,

it still turns out that the vast majority of people

have language on the left side, but it’s not 99%.

It’s more like 70%.

So if you’re left-handed, it’s still more likely

that the language part of your brain

is going to be on the left side,

but there’s going to be a greater proportion,

maybe 20, 30%, where it’s either in both hemispheres

or on the right side.

And just to make this a little bit more interesting

is that when people have strokes on the left side,

and if they’re lucky enough to recover from those strokes,

sometimes that involves reorganization,

this term that we call plasticity earlier,

where the areas around where the stroke

take on that new function

in a way that they didn’t have before.

That can certainly happen in the left hemisphere,

but there are also instances

where the right hemisphere

can also start to take on the function of language,

where when it was once on the left

and then transfers to the right.

So the thing that I think about a lot

is that the machinery probably exists on both sides,

but we don’t use them together all the time.

In fact, we may strongly bias one side or the other,

just like we use our two hands

in very, very different ways.

It’s a little bit the same with the brain.

Well, it’s because of what we do with the brain

that actually is why we use the hands in different ways.

And the same thing goes for language,

which is, again, the substrates, the organ,

the language organ, the part of the brain that processes it

probably has very similar machinery on the left side

as the right,

and the right may have the capability to do it,

but in real everyday use,

the brain specializes one of the sides

in order for us to use it functionally.

That’s a theory.

You’re bilingual, correct?

Yeah.

You speak English and Chinese?

Yeah.

For people that are bilingual

and that learn two or more, bilingual is two, obviously,

but learn both languages,

or let’s say more languages from an early time in life,

do they use the same brain area to generate that language,

or perhaps they use the left side to speak English

and the right side to speak Chinese?

Do we know anything about bilingualism in the brain?

I think we know a lot about bilingualism in the brain.

The answers are still out there, the final answers on it.

And part of the answer is yes, absolutely,

we use some parts of the brain.

Very similarly, we actually have a study in the lab right

now where we’re looking at this,

where people who speak one language or another,

or bilingual, and we’re looking at how the brain activity

patterns occur when they’re hearing one language

versus the other.

And what’s striking to see actually is how overlapping

they really can be,

even though the person may have no idea of the language

that they’re hearing,

the English part of the brain is still processing that

and maybe trying to interpret it through an English lens,

for example.

So the short answer is that with bilingualism,

there are shared circuitry, there’s a shared machinery

in the brain that allows us to process both,

but it’s not identical.

It’s the same part of the brain,

but what it’s doing with the signals can be very,

very different.

And what I mean by that precisely is not the instantaneous

detecting of one sound to the next,

but the memory of the sequences of those particular sounds

that give rise to things like words and meaning,

that can be highly variable from one individual to the next.

And those neurons are very,

very sensitive to the sequences of the sounds,

even though the sounds themselves might have some overlap

between languages.

Fascinating.

Okay, so we’ve talked about brain areas

and a little bit about lateralization.

I want to get back to the hands and some things related

to emotion in a little bit,

but maybe now we could go into those brain areas

and start to ask the question,

what exactly is represented or mapped there?

And for people who perhaps aren’t familiar with brain

mapping and representation and receptive fields,

perhaps the simplest analogy might be the visual system

where I look at your face, I know you, I recognize you.

And certainly there are brain areas that are responsible

for face recognition.

But the fact that I know that that’s your face.

And for those listening, I’m looking at Eddie’s face.

The fact that I know that that’s your face at all is because

we are well aware that there are cells that represent edges

and that represent dark and light.

And those all combine in what we call a hierarchical

structure.

They sort of build up from basic elements as simple as

little dots, but then lines and things that move, et cetera,

to give a coherent representation of the face.

When I think about language,

I think about words and just talking.

If I sit down to do a long podcast,

or I think about asking you a question,

I don’t even think about the words I want to say very much.

I mean, I have to think about them a little bit,

one would hope,

but I don’t think about individual syllables unless I’m

trying to, you know, accent something,

or it’s a word that I have a particular difficulty saying

where I want to change the cadence, et cetera.

So what’s represented in the neurons,

the nerve cells in these areas,

are they representing vowels, consonants?

And how do things like inflection,

like I occasionally will poke fun at up-speak,

but there’s a, I think a healthy,

a normal version of up-speak where somebody’s asking a

question, like for instance, what is that?

That’s an appropriate use of up-speak,

as opposed to saying something that is not a question and

putting a lilt at the end of the sentence,

then we call that up-speak,

which doesn’t fit with what the person is saying.

So what in the world is contained in these brain areas?

What is represented to me is perhaps one of the most

interesting questions.

And I know this lands square in your wheelhouse.

Sure. Let’s get into this, Andrew,

because this is one of the most exciting stuff that’s

happening right now is understanding how the brain processes

these exact questions.

And you asked me earlier, you know,

what is the difference between speech and language?

Speech corresponds to the communication signal.

It corresponds to me,

moving my mouth and my vocal tract to generate words.

And you’re hearing these as an auditory signal.

Language is something much broader.

So it refers to what you’re extracting from the words that

I’m saying.

We call that pragmatics and sort of,

are you getting the gist of what I’m saying?

There’s another aspect of it that we call semantics.

Do you understand the meaning of these words and the

sentences?

There’s another part that we call syntax,

which refers to how the words are assembled in a

grammatical form.

So those are all really critical parts of language.

And speech is just one form of language.

There’s many other forms like sign language, reading.

Those are all important modalities for reading.

Our research really focuses on this area that we’re

calling speech.

Again, the production of this audio signal,

which you can’t see, but your microphones are picking up.

There are these vibrations in the air that are created by my

vocal tract that are picked up by the microphone in the,

in the case of this recording,

but also picked up by the sensors in your ear,

the very tiny vibrations in your ear are picking that up and

translating that into electrical activity.

And what the ear does at the periphery is translates all

sounds into different frequencies.

So its main thing to do is to take a speech signal or any

other kind of sound and decompose it,

meaning separate that sound into different kind of signals.

And in the case of hearing,

what it’s doing is separating it out into low, middle,

high frequencies at a very, very high resolution.

It’s doing it very quickly and it’s doing it in a

really fine way to separate all of those different sounds.

So if you look at the periphery near the nerve that goes to

your ear, those nerve fibers,

some of them are tuned to low frequency.

Some of them are tuned to high frequency.

Some of them are tuned to the middle frequencies.

And that is what your ear is doing.

It’s taking these words and splitting them up into different

frequencies.

And for those of you out there that aren’t familiar with

thinking about things in the so-called frequency space,

bass tones would be lower frequencies and high pitch tones

would be higher frequencies,

just to make sure everyone’s on the same page.

So the sound of my voice, the sound of your voice,

or any sound in the environment is being broken down into

these frequencies.

Are they being broken down into very narrow channels of

frequency or are they, I want to avoid nomenclature here,

or are they being binned as fairly broad frequencies?

Because we know low, medium, and high, but for instance,

I can detect whether or not something’s approaching me or,

or moving away from me,

depending on whether or not it sweeps louder or right towards

our way, it’s subtle, but,

and of course it’s combined with what I see and my own

movement, but how finely sliced is our perception of the

auditory world?

Oh, extraordinarily precise.

I mean, we take these millisecond cues,

the millisecond differences between the sound coming to one

ear, let’s say your right ear versus your left,

to understand what direction that sound came from.

Those are only millisecond differences.

And that’s how precise this works.

But on the other hand, it does a lot of computation on this.

It does a lot of analysis as you go up.

And a lot of our work is focused on the part of the brain

that we call the cortex.

The cortex is the outermost part of the brain where we

believe that sounds are actually converted into words and

language.

So there’s this transformation where at the ear words are

decomposed and, you know,

turned into these elemental frequency channels.

And then as it goes up through the auditory system hits the

cortex, there are some things that happen,

obviously before it gets to the cortex,

but when it gets to cortex,

there’s something special going on,

which is that that part of the brain is looking for specific

sounds. And specifically what I mean by that is the sounds of

human language.

So the ones that are the different consonants and vowels

in a different language.

One of the ways that we have studied this is looking in

patients who have epilepsy.

And in a lot of these cases where the MRI looks completely

normal,

we have to put electrodes surgically on a part of the

brain. The temporal lobe is a very, very common place.

So we’ve done a lot of our work looking at how the temporal

lobe processes speech sounds,

because we’re looking for where the seizures start,

but then we’re also doing brain mapping for language and

speech. So we can protect those areas.

We want to identify the areas that we want to remove to cure

someone’s seizures,

but we also want to figure out the areas that are important

for speech and language to protect those so that we can do

a surgery that’s effective and safe.

And so in our research and why it’s become a really

important addition to our knowledge is that we have

electrodes directly recording from the human brain surface.

A lot of technology we work with right now is recording on

the mill on the order of millimeters and they can order,

they can record millisecond time resolution of neural

activity.

And what we see is extraordinary patterns of

activity. When people hear words and sentences,

if you look at that part of the brain that we call Wernicke’s

area in this part of the temporal lobe,

this whole area lights up when you hear words or speech.

And it’s not in a way that is like a general light bulb

warming up and it’s generally lit up.

But what you actually see is something much,

much more complicated, which is a pattern of activity.

And what we’ve done in the last 10 years is to try to

understand what does that pattern come from?

And if we were to look at each individual site from that part

of the brain, what would we see?

What parts of words are being coded by electrical activity

in those parts of the brain?

Remember the cortex is using electrical activity to

transmit information and do analysis.

And what we’re doing is we’re eavesdropping on this part of

the brain as it’s processing speech to try to understand

what each individual site is doing.

And what are those sites doing?

Or could you give us some examples of what those sites are

doing? So for instance,

are they sites that are specific for,

or we could say even listening for consonants or for vowels

or for inflection or for emotionality, what’s in there?

Okay. Well, what makes these, what,

what makes these cells fire?

Yeah. What gets them excited?

What gets them going is hearing speech in particular.

There are some of these really focal sites, again,

just on the order of millimeter or at some level,

single neurons that are tuned to consonants.

Some are tuned to vowels.

Some are tuned to particular features of consonants.

What I mean by that are different categories of consonants.

There’s a class of consonants that we call plosive

consonants. There’s a little bit of linguistic jargon,

but I’m going to make a point here with that is that certain

classes of sounds, when you make them,

it requires you to actually close your mouth temporarily.

Now I’m going to be thinking about this.

So plosive, like plosive,

like saying the word plosive does requires that.

Exactly. So what’s cool about that is that we actually have

no idea what’s going on in our mouth. When we speak,

we really have no idea.

Some people definitely have no idea.

Well, not just like in terms of what you’re saying

sometimes, but actually like how you’re actually moving,

right. You know, the different parts of vocal track.

And I have a feeling if we actually required understanding,

we would never be able to speak because it’s so complex.

It’s such a complex feat.

Some people would say it’s the most complex motor thing that

we do as a species is just speaking, not, you know,

the extreme feats of acrobatics or athleticism,

but speaking.

Well, and especially when one observes, you know,

opera or people who, you know, freestyle rappers, you know,

and of course it’s not just the lips, it’s the tongue.

Yeah. And, and you’ve mentioned two other structures,

pharynx and larynx are the main ones that,

can you tell us just,

just educate us at a superficial level,

what the pharynx and larynx do differentially?

Because I think most people aren’t going to be familiar.

Sure.

So I’ll talk primarily about the larynx here for a second,

which is that if you think about when we’re speaking,

really what we’re doing is we’re shaping the breath.

So even before you get to the larynx,

you got to start with the expiration.

So we fill up our lungs and then we push the air out.

That’s a normal part of breathing.

And what is really amazing about speech and language

is that we evolved to take advantage

of that normal physiologic thing at a larynx.

And what the larynx does is that when you’re exhaling,

it brings the vocal folds together.

Some people call them vocal cords.

They’re not really cords.

They’re really vocal folds.

They’re two pieces of tissue that come together

and a muscle brings them together.

And then what happens is when the air comes

through the vocal folds, when they’re together,

they vibrate at really high frequencies,

like 100 to 200 Hertz.

Yours is probably about 100 Hertz.

The average-

Yours is 200?

No, no.

Most male voices are around 100, okay?

And then the average female voice is around 200 Hertz.

And as you know, I’ve always had the same voice.

This was a point of shame when I was a kid.

Folks, my voice never changed.

I always had the same voice.

This is a discussion for another time.

Yeah, well, it’s a great voice, a great baritone voice,

but I know in your voice, it’s a low-frequency voice.

And the reason why men and women

generally have different voice qualities

is it has to do with the size of the larynx

and the shape of it.

Okay, so in general, men have a larger voice box

or larynx, and the vibrating frequency,

the resonance frequency of the vocal folds

when the air comes through them

is about 100 Hertz for men and about 200 for women.

So what happens is, okay, so you’re taking,

you take a breath in, and then as the air is coming out,

the vocal folds come together and the air goes through.

That creates the sound of the voice that we call voicing.

And that’s the energy of your voice.

It’s not just your voice characteristic.

It’s the energy of your voice.

It’s coming from the larynx there.

It’s a noise.

And then it’s the source of the voice.

And then what happens is that energy,

that sound goes up through the parts of the vocal tract,

like the pharynx into the oral cavity,

which is your mouth and your tongue and your lips.

And what those things are doing

is that they’re shaping the air in particular ways

that create consonants and vowels.

So that’s what I mean by shaping the breath.

It just starts with this exhalation.

You generate the voice in the larynx,

and then everything above the larynx is moving around,

just like the way my mouth is doing right now,

to shape that air into particular patterns

that you can hear as words.

Fascinating.

And immediately makes me wonder about more primitive

or non-learned vocalizations like crying or laughter.

Babies will cry.

Babies will show laughter.

Are those sorts of vocalizations

produced by the language areas like Wernicke’s,

or do they have their own unique neural structures?

Yeah, interesting question.

So we call those vocalizations.

A vocalization is basically where someone can create a sound

like a cry or a moan, that kind of sound.

And it also involves the exhalation of air.

It also involves some phonation at the level of the larynx

where the vocal folds come together

to create that audible sound.

But it turns out that those are actually different areas.

So people who have injuries

in the speech and language areas

oftentimes can still moan, they can still vocalize.

And it is a different part of the brain.

I would say an area that even non-human primates have

that can be specialized for vocalization.

It’s a different form of communication

than words, for example.

The intricacy of these circuits in the brain

and their connections to the pharynx and larynx

is just, it’s almost overwhelming

in terms of thinking about just how complicated it must be.

And yet some general features and principles

are starting to emerge from your work

and from the work of others.

If we think about that work and we think about,

for instance, Wernicke’s area,

if I were to record from neurons in Wernicke’s area

at different locations,

would I find that there’s any kind of systematic layout,

for instance, in terms of,

we’ve talked about sound frequency.

We know that low frequencies are represented

at one end of a structure

and high frequencies at the other.

This is true, actually, at least from my earlier training

within the year itself, within the cochlea,

early work of von Bekesey and from cadavers, right?

They actually figured this out from dead people,

which is incredible, a fascinating literature

people should look up.

And in the visual system, we know that, for instance,

you know, visual position,

where things are is mapped systematically.

In other words, neurons that sit next to each other

in the brain represent portions of visual space

that are next to each other in the real world.

What is the organization of language

in areas like Wernicke’s and Broca’s?

For instance, I think of the vowels, A-E-I-O-U,

as a kind of a coherent unit,

but do I find the A neurons are next to the E neurons

are next to the, or the A-E-I-O-U?

Is that vowel representation also laid out in order

or is it kind of salt and pepper?

Is it random?

That’s been one of the like most important questions

we’ve been trying to answer for the past decade.

So there is a part of the brain

that we call the primary auditory cortex,

and the primary auditory cortex is deep

in the temporal lobe.

And if you looked at that part of the brain,

there is a map of different sound frequencies.

So if you look at the front of that primary auditory cortex,

you’ll find low frequency sounds.

And then as you march backwards in that cortex,

it goes from low to medium to high frequencies.

It’s organized in this really nice and orderly way.

And it turns out there’s just not just one,

there’s like mirrors of that tone frequency map

in the primary auditory cortex.

The areas that are really important for speech

are on the side of that.

And we now think that speech can go straight

to the speech cortex without having to go through

the primary auditory cortex,

that it has its own pathway to get to the part of the brain

that processes speech.

And when we’ve looked at that question about,

is there a map?

The short answer is yes, there is a map.

And it is, but it is not structured universally

across all people in a way that we can clearly see right now.

It is like a salt and pepper map

of the different features in speech.

So before we talked about these sounds

that are called plosives,

you make a plosive when the mouth

or something in the oral cavity closes temporarily.

And when it opens, that creates that fast plosive sound.

So when you say dad or, you know, the ball,

like the B in ball, that kind of thing,

you will notice that your lips actually close

and then it’s the release of that

that creates that particular sounds.

Okay, so those are the sounds that we call plosives.

Those are like bada ga, pata ka.

Those are a certain class of consonants

that we call plosive sounds.

There’s another class of sounds

that we call fricatives in linguistics.

Fricatives are created by turbulence

in the airstream as it comes out through the mouth.

And the way that we make that turbulence

is getting the mouth and the lips to close

almost until they’re completely shut.

Or putting the tongue to near the teeth

to almost get it completely shut,

but just have a narrow aperture.

That creates a turbulence in the airflow

that we perceive as a high-frequency sound.

So those are the sounds like sha and tha,

those kind of things.

If you look at the frequencies,

they’re higher frequencies

and those are created by specific movements

that you constrict the airflow to create turbulence.

And we hear it as sha, sa, tha.

So if I say that.

Exactly.

And as opposed to a plosive where I’d say,

explosive.

Now, of course I’m emphasizing here.

Well, this explains something and solves a mystery,

which is recently I’ve been fascinated

by the work of a physician scientist back East,

Dr. Shana Swan, who’s done a lot of work

on things that are contained in pesticides

and foods that are changing hormone levels.

And she refers to phthalates, which is spelled P.

So it’s both a plosive and a tha.

So it’s combining the two.

And it’s one of the most difficult words

in the English language to pronounce.

And second only perhaps to the correct pronunciation

of ophthalmology.

So it’s a combination of a plosive

and one of these tha sounds.

And that’s probably why it’s difficult.

That’s exactly right.

In fact, we have a term for that.

That’s called a consonant cluster.

So sometimes syllables will just have one consonant,

but when we start stacking certain syllables in a sequence

and there’s rules that actually govern

which consonants can be in a particular sequence

for a given language, that makes it more complicated.

And certain languages have a lot more consonant clusters

than others.

So for instance, Russian, for example,

has a lot of constant clusters.

English has a lot of them.

There are other languages that have very, very few.

For example, Hawaiian.

Hawaiian has an inventory of about 12

to 14 different phonemes,

14 different consonants and vowels.

English, on contrast, has about 40

different consonants and vowels.

So languages have different inventories.

They can overlap for sure,

but different languages use different sound elements,

combine and recombine those elements

to give rise to different words and meanings.

Can we say that there’s a most complicated language

out there or among the most complicated,

would it be Russian?

It’s definitely high up there.

English is up there too, actually.

Yeah, German as well.

And in terms of learning multiple languages

during development,

my understanding is that if one wants to become bilingual

or trilingual, best to learn those languages

simultaneously during development,

ideally before age 12,

if one hopes to not have an accent in speaking them later.

Is that correct?

Or do you want to revise that?

Well, basically the earlier,

and the earlier is better,

the more intense it is and the more immersive it is,

the longer that you can be exposed to that

is really important.

A lot of people can get exposed to early

and basically lose it,

even though it’s quote unquote during that sensitive period,

unless it’s maintained, it can be very easily lost.

Then I think another aspect of it that’s very interesting

is some of the social requirements for it too.

It’s pretty clear that you can only go so far

just listening to these sounds from a tape recording

or something like that.

There’s something extra about real human interactions

that activates the brain sensitivity

to different speech bounds,

allows us to become specialized for them

for a given language.

So returning to what’s mapped,

what the representations are in the brain,

I’m starting to get a picture now

based on these plosives and these sounds.

And what I find so interesting and logical about that

is it maps to the motor structures

and the actual pronunciation of the sounds,

not necessarily to the meaning of the individual words.

Now, of course, it’s related to the meaning

of the individual words,

but it makes good sense to me

why something as complex as language,

both to understand and to generate

would map to something that is essentially motor in design,

because as you point out,

I have to generate these sounds

and I have to hear them generated from others.

However, there’s reading and there’s writing

and writing is certainly motor,

reading involves some motor commands of the eyes, et cetera.

Where do reading and writing come into this picture?

Are they in parallel with, as we would say in neuroscience,

or are they embedded within the same structures?

Are they part of the same series of computations?

Yeah, so to address the first part

is that we’ve got this map

of these different parts of consonants and vowels.

And when we look at how they lay out

in this part of the brain that we call Wernicke’s area,

we’ve spent a lot of time

really just dissecting this millimeter by millimeter.

The term that you use is very apropos.

It’s salt and pepper.

It’s not random.

There is this kind of selectivity

to these individual speech sounds.

And one point I want to make about it is this,

is that in English, for example,

there are about 40 different phonemes.

Phonemes are just consonants or vowels

or individual speech segments.

But these articulatory features that you refer to,

for example, the characteristic sounds

that are generated by specific movements in the mouth,

you can more or less reduce that

to about 12 different features.

Okay, these are specific movements of the tongue,

the jaw, the lips, the larynx.

There are about 12 of these movements.

And just like you said, Andrew,

by themselves, they have no meaning.

They’re just movements.

But what’s incredible about it

is that you take these 12 movements

and you put them in combinations

and you start putting them in sequence.

We as humans use those 12 set of features

to generate all words.

And because we can generate

nearly an infinite number of words

with that code of just 12 features,

we have something that generates

essentially all possible meaning.

Because that’s what we do as humans.

We generate meanings.

I’m trying to communicate one idea to another,

which to me is extraordinary.

A parallel would be, for example, DNA.

There’s four base pairs in DNA,

but with those four base pairs in a specific sequence

can generate an entire code for life.

And speech is the same way.

It’s like you’ve got these fundamental elements

that by themselves have no meaning,

but when you put them together,

give rise to every possible meaning.

So with regard to your second point

about reading and writing,

it’s a fascinating question.

Speech and language is part of who we are as humans.

That’s part of how we evolved

and it’s hardwired and molded by experience.

Reading and writing are a human invention.

It’s something that was added on

to the architecture of the brain.

And because reading and writing

are fairly recent in human evolution,

it’s essentially too quick for anything

to have a dramatic change in,

let’s say, a new brain area

or some kind of specialization.

Instead, what happens is that whenever

any kind of behavior becomes ultra-specialized

in any of us or any organism,

we can take some areas that are normally involved

with vision, for example,

and specialize it for the purpose of reading.

So all of us have a part of our brain

in the back of the temporal lobe

that interfaces with the occipital visual cortex

that we call a visual word form area.

There’s actually a part of the brain

that is very sensitive to seeing words,

like either typed or handwritten.

There’s a part of the brain

that also is sensitive to seeing things like faces.

So these are things that are all conditioned

on what’s important to survive.

So reading and writing are an invention,

and there are things that have mapped

to functions that the brain already has.

And one of the really important things

about reading and writing is that

when we learn to read and write,

especially with the reading part,

it maps to the part of the brain

that we’ve been talking about,

which is the part that’s processing speech sounds.

So some of us kind of think about it.

These are two different things.

One is hearing sounds through your ears.

The other is reading,

where you’re actually seeing things through your eyes

and then getting into languages.

Well, it turns out that the auditory speech cortex

is the primal and primitive fundamental area

that’s really important for speech.

And what happens with the reading

is once it gets through that visual cortex,

it’s going to try to map those reading signals

to the part of the brain

that’s trying to make sense of sounds,

the sounds of words, what we call phonology.

Now, why is this important?

It has a lot of relevance to how we learn to write.

And in some kids with dyslexia,

dyslexia is a neurological condition

where a child, in some cases an adult,

has trouble reading, for example.

And in many of those cases,

it’s because that mapping between how we see the words

to the way that the brain processes the sounds

is something different.

It’s a little bit different

than people who can read really well.

So when you’re reading, a lot of times,

you’re actually activating the part of the brain

that is processing the words that you hear.

What is the current treatment for dyslexia?

I’ve heard that it’s a deficit

in some of the motion processing systems

of the visual system.

People, their eyes are jumping

as opposed to more linear reading across,

or I suppose if it were Chinese, it would be,

I want to presume people are always reading English,

or I suppose if it’s Hebrew,

they’re going from the opposite side of the page.

What can be done for dyslexia?

And do any of the modern treatments for dyslexia

involve changing things from the speech side

as opposed to just the quote-unquote reading side,

given that speech and reading are interconnected?

Yeah, absolutely.

So again, I think in the beginning,

people might’ve thought this was purely a visual abstraction

or something really just about the visual system,

but there’s been more recognition that it could be both,

or it could be either,

depending on the particular instance.

It’s very clear that there are many kids with dyslexia

where the problem is a problem of phonological awareness.

So it can be very hard to detect

because they may understand the words that you were saying,

but because the brain is so good at pattern recognition,

sometimes even if the individual speech sounds

are not crystal clear, it can compensate that

so that you can have an individual who can hear the words,

but not be able to essentially hear them

when they’re reading those same words.

And so what can happen with that

is that you can have this disconnection

between what they’re seeing and what they need

in order to hear it as words and process it as language.

And so skilled readers usually need that route first.

They’ve got to map the vision to the sound

in order to get that sort of like foundation.

But then over time, the reading has its direct connection

to the language parts of the brain.

And we don’t necessarily always need to map to sounds.

You can basically develop a parallel route.

And we as readers actually use both all the time.

So for example, if it’s a new word

that you’ve never seen before,

sometimes you try to like pronounce it in your mind

and try to hear what that word is.

Even though you’re not actually saying it,

you’re trying to just generate

what those sounds might be like.

And that’s the part where we’re kind of relying

on how we learn to read in the first place,

which is mapping those word images

to the sounds that go along with them.

But in other times, if you’re a really proficient reader,

you’re just seeing the words

and you can map them directly to meaning

without having to go through that process.

Yeah, I’m a big fan of listening to audio books.

And of course, I also listen to podcasts quite a lot,

but I also am a strong believer

based on the research that I’ve seen

that reading books, physical books,

could be on Kindle, I suppose,

but reading a physical book is useful

for being able to articulate well and structure sentences

and build what are essentially paragraphs,

which is what I’m required to do

when I do solo episodes of the podcast.

I’ve noticed over the years

as text messaging has become more popular

and there’s essentially an erosion of punctuation

or the need to have complete sentences.

And now that’s sort of transferred to email as well.

It’s become acceptable to just say, you know,

fragmented sentences in email.

And it seems likely that it’s starting to impact

the way that people speak as well.

And I don’t think this has anything to do

with intelligence or education level,

but are you aware of any evidence

that how we read and what we read

and whether or not we consume information

purely through reading or mainly through auditory sources,

does it change the way that we speak?

Because after all, Wernicke’s and Broca’s area

and the other auditory and speech production areas

are heavily intermeshed.

And so it would make perfect sense to me

that what we hear and the patterns of sound

that are being communicated to us

would also change the way that we speak.

Yeah, that’s a really fascinating point.

There’s this idea that there’s like this proper way

to speak, like that there’s the right way, for example,

what are the appropriate, you know,

like, for example, in school,

you’re oftentimes told like,

you should say like this, not say like that, you know,

and every language kind of has that.

It turns out that that’s really unnatural.

Languages and speech in particular change over time,

it evolves and it can happen very quickly.

You know, the things that we call dialects, for example,

are just different ways of speaking

and someone can just be in one environment

and change from one dialect to the other

or in some people, it kind of is really fixed.

And there’s this idea that, you know, like in school,

that we’re like told that there’s this right way,

but in reality, that’s not true.

Like language change and speech change

is completely normal and happens all the time.

And it can be really dramatic,

like certain cultures and communities,

if they are isolated, they can develop a whole new language,

a whole new set of words, for example,

and new ways and dialects that are independent

from people to the point where it’s unintelligible

even to others.

And so the basic idea is that sound change

is part of the way it works.

And the brain is very sensitive to those kinds of changes.

Speaking of learning new languages,

I’m assuming it’s possible to learn new languages

throughout the lifespan, correct?

Yeah.

I’ve also heard these kind of fantastical stories

of somebody has a stroke and then suddenly, spontaneously,

can speak French fluently,

whereas prior to the stroke, they could not.

Is there any merit to those stories whatsoever?

I find it very hard to believe

that there was a complete map representation of a language

in somebody’s brain that they were completely unaware of.

And then because of damage to a brain area,

that capacity to speak that language was somehow unveiled.

It just seems too wild.

And I don’t want to say good to be true

because nobody wants a stroke,

but it just seems outrageously implausible.

Well, there are aspects of that

that certainly are implausible.

So I don’t know of any true case that I’ve ever seen

or experienced myself or even read about where,

for example, there was an injury to the brain

that resulted in loss of,

well, essentially a gain of function,

meaning just all of a sudden

started speaking another language.

So for example, if you had a stroke

and you never spoke French, and then you had it,

and then all of a sudden you’re speaking,

that I’ve never heard of, never seen.

However, there is a condition that is well-acknowledged,

and I have seen one case of this

called a foreign accent syndrome,

which is peculiar because there are people

who have an injury to the part of the brain

where it sounds like they’re starting

to speak this other language,

but they’re not actually speaking the language.

It just sounds like it.

And this goes back to what we were talking about earlier

about these areas that are really important

for speech control of the vocal tract,

this area in the precentral gyrus.

People have documented where patients have had strokes there

and after that, it sounds like they’re speaking Spanish

as opposed to English,

or it sounds like they have the intonational properties

of French or Russian

as compared to their original native language.

They’re not learning all the rest of it,

like the meaning and the grammar, et cetera,

but they’re adopting some of the phonology,

and part of that is just because it’s not working

the way it normally does.

So there is something actually called

a foreign accent syndrome

that people can have after a stroke.

Interesting.

I’m curious about auditory memory.

When I was a kid, I used to get into bed at night

and I’d close my eyes

and I would replay conversations

that I’d heard during the day or people’s voices.

I actually can remember calling your house

when we were young kids,

because I don’t speak any Chinese,

but I’d have to ask for you.

I’d say, I think it was Eddie Saibutsai.

Yeah. Yeah.

And then someone, whoever answered the phone would say,

would go get you and then I’d say,

which I believe means thank you, right?

That’s the total of the Chinese that I speak, by the way,

but I will never forget that.

I’ll just never forget it.

I hope, I suppose if I have a stroke

or something of that sort,

at some point I’ll forget it

and I won’t know that I’ve forgotten it,

but in all seriousness, I remember that to this day.

I couldn’t spell that out.

I wouldn’t know how, certainly not in Chinese,

but even a transliteration I couldn’t do

using English letters.

Where are memories of sounds stored?

Because within our days and across our lives,

we have a infinite number of auditory experiences,

just like we have an infinite number of visual experiences.

Where are they stored?

And what is the structure of their storage?

What am I calling upon?

Besides of course, the motor commands

that are required to say what I just said in Chinese,

which I won’t repeat again,

because I can somehow manage to get it right

the first time, or at least not terribly wrong,

then I don’t want to botch it the second time.

Where is that stored?

And how does that work?

And more importantly, as I speak my native language,

English, am I pulling from a memory bank?

Because it doesn’t feel like it.

I’m just telling you what I want to say.

I’m doing my best to communicate clearly and succinctly.

Usually not so good at the succinct part,

but where is the bank of information?

On my keyboard, on my computer, I have the letters

and I have certain elements of punctuation in the space.

What am I pulling from?

Am I pulling from those plosives?

But if so, how can I do it so quickly?

Even for people that speak slowly,

it appears more or less fluid.

This to me is overwhelmingly impressive

that the brain can do that.

How does it do that?

Well, first of all, I am impressed that 35 years later.

Well, I had to get ahold of you.

Yeah, so I am impressed 35 years later

that you can still remember that.

But only that.

That’s fine.

But I’m still very impressed in,

but clearly it was something important to you.

And so the short answer is that memory is very distributed.

So it’s almost like the question that you asked me

is ill-posed, because you asked me where.

Well, it’s not one specific area.

It’s actually really distributed.

It’s not just one particular area.

In fact, I’m fairly certain that if we were to injure

that part of the brain called the Wernicke’s area,

you may still even have memories of that.

People can have injuries of Broca’s area

or certainly the precentral gyrus

and be able to sing happy birthday, for example,

when it’s embedded in melody or highly rehearsed things

like counting, despite not being able to speak,

which is incredible, right?

It’s like, you can see a patient, for example,

who can’t really put together a sentence.

You ask them, how are you feeling today?

They can’t even utter a word,

but then you ask them to count sometimes

and they’ll get up to any number, really.

And so there are some things that are really built

into our motor memory and it’s distributed.

It’s not one particular part of the brain.

It’s actually multiple areas

where that memory is distributed.

And thank God that’s the way it is,

because it’s very rare in the kind of surgeries that I do

where you go and you remove a part or piece of the brain

that someone forgets these kind of long-term memories

or these long-term motor skills that they have.

That’s very, very rare.

It’s the number one question a patient will ask me,

like, am I going to be the same

and am I going to remember my wife?

Or am I going to remember these thoughts of my birthday

when I was 10 years old?

And I’ve never really seen that kind of severe amnesia,

unless it’s a very, very severe injury

that involves almost the entire brain.

And thank God.

So a lot of that information is really distributed

across the entire brain.

Speaking of storage of and ability to speak,

you are doing some amazing work

and have achieved some pretty incredible,

well-deserved recognition for your work

in bringing language out of paralyzed people,

essentially allowing people who are locked in

to a paralyzed state

or otherwise unable to articulate speech

using brain machine interface,

essentially translating the neural activity

of areas of the brain that would produce speech

into hardware, wires and things of that sort,

artificial non-biological tools

in order to allow paralyzed people to communicate.

And we will provide a link

to some of the popular press coverage of that work

in the original papers.

But if you would be so kind as to tell us

what those experiments look like,

who these people are who are locked in

and that you allow to communicate.

And then especially interesting to me

is some of the directions that you’re taking this now,

which is beyond just people being able to think

about what they want to say

and words coming out on a screen or through a microphone,

but actually making the interactions

between these people in the real world

more elaborate and more real.

If that seems mysterious to people,

I’m going to let Eddie tell you

what they’re doing with this

rather than put any more detail on it.

Oh, okay.

Well, thanks for asking about this.

This has really been some of the exciting recent work

from the lab.

So for the last decade,

we’ve really been focusing on the basic science,

meaning trying to understand how the brain extracts

and produces speech sounds and words.

We’ve done a lot of work trying to figure out

how these parts of the brain

control these individual elements

that give rise to all words and meanings.

And so it was about six years ago

where we realized we actually have a pretty good idea

of how this code works.

We had identified all of these different elements

that we could decode in epilepsy patients.

For example, when they had electrodes on the brain

as part of their surgeries,

we could decode all of the different consonants

and vowels of English.

That was about six years ago.

So a natural question was this,

which is if we understand that electrical code,

can we use that to help someone who is paralyzed

and can’t get those signals out of the brain

to speak normally?

And that’s in the setting of people who are paralyzed.

So there are a series of conditions.

They include things like brainstem stroke.

The brainstem is the part of the brain

that connects the cerebrum,

which is the top part, does our thinking

and a lot of the motor control,

speech, language, everything.

And the brainstem is what connects that to the spinal cord

and the nerves that go out to the face and vocal tract.

So if you have a stroke there,

basically you could be thinking all the wild,

creative, intelligent thoughts you have in the mind

and the cerebrum, but you can’t get them out into words

or you can’t get them out to your hand to write them down.

So that’s a very severe form of paralysis

called brainstem stroke.

There’s another kind of conditions

that we call neurodegenerative,

where the nerve cells die basically or atrophy

in a condition called ALS.

And that’s a very severe form of paralysis.

In its extreme form,

people essentially lose all voluntary movement.

So Stephen Hawking would be a good example

of someone with ALS, Lou Gehrig’s disease.

He’s an example of someone who had ALS,

but not a great example of what typical course of ALS.

So for reasons not clear,

the progression of his disease largely stabilized

to the point where he could twitch a cheek muscle

or move his eyes, let’s say.

And most people, it’s very rapid.

And many people, they die from it actually,

within a couple of years of diagnosis.

Yeah, he lived a long time in that.

He lived a long time.

That slanted overstay in his wheelchair.

Exactly.

But he wasn’t breathing through a tube in his throat,

for example, because people with severe ALS,

the muscles to their diaphragm and their lungs

essentially give out as well.

They get weakness there

and then they can’t breathe anymore.

So that’s another form of paralysis.

And so in our field,

these are kind of like the most devastating things

that can happen.

I’m not going to really try to compare like what’s worse,

you know, having a brain tumor or stroke, it’s all bad.

But this condition of what we call being locked in

refers to this idea

that you can have completely intact cognition and awareness,

but have no way to express that.

No voluntary movement, no ability to speak.

And that is devastating because psychologically

and socially, you’re completely isolated.

That’s what we call locked in syndrome.

And it’s devastating.

I’ve seen that throughout my career.

And it’s really heartbreaking because

you know that the person is there,

but you can’t see, they can’t communicate.

So we’ve been studying this patterning

of electrical activity for consonants and vowels.

And essentially, once we figured out a lot of these codes

for the individual phonetic elements,

we took a little bit of a detour,

or at least part of the lab started to focus

on this very specific question.

For people who have these kinds of paralysis,

could we intercept those signals from the brain,

the cerebral cortex,

as someone is trying to say those words?

And then can we intercept them

and then have them taken out of the brain

through wires to a computer

that are gonna interpret those signals

and translate them into words?

So about three years ago, we started a clinical trial.

It’s called the BRAVO trial.

It’s still underway.

And the first participant in the BRAVO trial

was a man who had been paralyzed for 15 years.

When he was about 20 years old,

he came to the United States,

was actually working in Sonoma area,

and he was in a car accident.

And he actually walked out of the hospital

the day after that car accident.

But the next day had a complication related to it,

where he had a very large stroke in the brainstem.

And that turned out to be devastating.

He didn’t wake up from that stroke for about a week.

He was in a coma for about a week.

And when he woke up from that coma,

he realized that he couldn’t speak

or move his arms or legs.

And as he told me or communicated to us,

that was absolutely devastating.

He wanted really to die at that time.

Could he blink his eyes or move his mouth in any way?

He could blink his eyes.

He had some limited mouth movements,

but couldn’t produce any intelligible speech.

He was like completely slurred and incomprehensible.

And he survived this injury.

A lot of people who have that kind of stroke

just don’t survive.

But he survived.

And I also realized that he’s just an incredible person,

like a force of nature in terms of his optimism,

in terms of his ability to make friends

despite his condition.

The way he actually communicates,

because he has a little bit of residual neck movements,

is that he improvised and had his friends

basically put a stick attached to his baseball cap.

Because he could move his neck,

he would essentially type out letters

on a keyboard screen to get out words.

In fact, this is how he communicated,

was through a device that he would essentially

peck out letters one by one by moving his neck

to control this stick attached to his baseball cap.

How many years did he use that method of communication?

For about 15 years.

He hadn’t really spoken for about 15 years.

Oh, goodness.

Yeah.

So it was a devastating injury,

but there’s something to be said about the human spirit.

And if there’s anyone who embodies it, it is Pancho.

That’s his nickname, the first participant in our trial.

He has that human spirit.

He persevered and, in fact,

could thrive in his community, basically, and friends,

being able to communicate

in this very slow and inefficient way.

Maybe part of that spirit is why he volunteered

to be the first person in this trial.

It was a clinical trial, an experiment.

It was a study.

This is not an approved therapy by any means.

This was really something that had not been done before.

And we had a lot of ideas about it,

but we didn’t know.

We had proven a lot of this could be true

in some people who are normally speaking,

but to actually put into someone who’s paralyzed,

number one, where we don’t know the code is the same.

Number two is someone who’s not been speaking for 15 years,

whether those signals are actually still there or not.

So it was part of a clinical trial.

It was something that our hospital

and also the FDA had to approve

and looked at very carefully.

But given a lot of the work that we had done,

there was some basis for why this might work.

And so about two and a half years ago,

we did a surgery where we implanted electrodes

onto the parts of the brain that we’ve been talking about,

these areas that control the vocal tract,

the areas that control the larynx,

the areas that control the lips and tongue

and jaw movements when we normally speak.

These are areas that presumably may be active.

That was our hope in his brain,

but he just couldn’t get those out

to control his mouth in a normal way.

And he underwent a surgery, a brain surgery,

where we put an electrode array

and we connected it to a port

that was screwed to his skull.

The port actually goes through his scalp

and he’s lived with this now for the last three years.

It is a risk of infection.

These ports eventually have to become wireless in the future

but we figured out a way to keep that port there

where we can essentially connect him to a computer

through that port.

So he has an electrode array that’s implanted

over the part of his brain that’s important for speech.

It’s connected to a port.

And then we connect a wire to that port

that translates those, what we call analog brainwaves

and converts them into digital signals.

And then a computer takes those digital signals

from those individual sites from the speech cortex

and translates those into words.

Can you describe for us the first time

that Poncho spoke through this engineered device?

What was that experience like for you?

And at least from what he conveyed to you,

what was that experience like for him?

Is this somebody who was essentially locked in

except for this rather crude pecking device?

Although I’m thoroughly impressed by how adaptive

or adaptable Poncho was in his friends

engineering that device for him

was really nothing short of clever

because otherwise he would be truly locked in.

But what was that moment like?

I can only imagine.

That moment was incredible.

It was truly incredible to be able to see him

try to get out a word that was for all practical purposes

unintelligible, but to be able to take the brain activity

and to translate it into text on a screen.

That’s what we did.

We took those brainwaves.

We put them through machine learning

or artificial intelligence algorithm

that can pick up these very, very subtle patterns.

You can actually see them with your eye

in the brain activity and translate those into words.

And I remember seeing this happening for the first time.

It doesn’t happen like immediately.

This is something that took weeks to train the algorithm

to interpret it correctly.

But what was incredible about it was to see how he reacted.

And he would be prompted to say a given word

like, you know, outside, for example.

And then he would think about it, try to say it.

And finally those words would appear on the screen.

And what was really amazing about it was

you could really tell that he like got a kick out of that

because he would start to giggle.

You know, his body would shake in a way

and his head would shake in a way

that he would start to giggle.

And that was cool to see.

But then I also realized that when he was giggling,

it kind of screwed up the next words decoding.

Is that a bug you’ve since fixed?

No, we haven’t fixed that.

Interesting.

We haven’t fixed that.

So it’s easier just to tell him to stop giggling.

So what was the first word that he said?

Well, I think one of the first sentences

that he put together was, you know,

can you get my family outside?

And-

Meaning get them out of the room?

No, no.

All these years you wanted to get away from his family?

No, I think what he meant was, can you get them?

Bring them in.

Bring them in.

And so the way this worked was we trained

this computer to recognize 50 words.

We started with a very small vocabulary

that’s expanding as we speak.

I think that this is just a matter of time

before these vocabularies become much, much larger.

But we started with a 50 set of words.

We created essentially all the possible sentences

that you could generate from those 50 words.

Why that was important was you can use

all those possible sentences to create

a computational model, computer model

of all the different word combinations

to give different sentences given those 50 words.

And then you can essentially do what we call autocorrect.

It’s the same kind of thing that we do

when you’re texting, for example,

you get the wrong letter in there,

but your phone actually knows, you know,

because it’s context what to correct it.

So because the decoding is not 100% correct all the time,

in fact, it’s far from that,

it’s really helpful to have these other features

like autocorrect, the stuff that we use routinely now

with texting that makes it correct and then updates it.

So it’s a combination of a lot of things.

It’s the AI that is translating

those brain activity patterns,

but it’s also things that we’ve learned from speech

and speech technologies that, you know,

you put all together and then all of a sudden

it starts to work.

And so we were really excited

because that was the first time that someone was paralyzed

and could create words and sentences

that was just decoded from the brain activity.

Incredible.

And I know you’re very humble,

but I’m going to embarrass you by saying

I always knew you were destined for great things

since the early age of nine when we first became friends.

But when I read that,

the news coverage of your work with Poncho

and the release of this language

from this locked in patient,

it literally, you know, it brought tears to my eyes

because it’s, you know,

it’s an interesting thing as fellow neuroscientists, right?

We explore the brain and we try and find mechanisms

and we try and compare those to what other people find

and find truths and principles and build up from those.

But pretty rarely is there a case where

that route of exploration leads

to something of clinical significance

within one’s own lifetime.

I mean, that’s the reality of science.

And oftentimes it’s a very distributed process.

But in this case,

it’s been a magnificent thing to see you

move along this trajectory,

parsing these language and speech areas,

and then to also do the clinical work in parallel.

Speaking of which,

these days we hear a lot about Neuralink,

Elon Musk’s company,

a neurosurgeon that came up briefly through my lab,

but I can’t take any credit for what he knows or does,

which is Matt McDougall is the neurosurgeon at Neuralink.

There’s some other excellent neuroscientists there

and engineers there.

We hear a lot about Neuralink

because while brain machine interface

of the sort that you do and that other laboratories do

has been going on for a long time,

there’s been some press around Neuralink

about the promise of what brain machine interface could do.

For instance, early in our discussion,

you talked about how language is constrained

by these sound waves

and typically it’s a few people communicating

or one person with many people

through a podcast, for instance, or a speech.

But the idea has been thrown out there

that through the use of stimulating chips

or through other brain machine devising

that perhaps one could internalize

50 conversations in parallel, right?

50X communication.

Or that the memory systems could be augmented

to remember 10 times as much information

or even twice as much information in a given period of time.

My understanding of what they’re doing at Neuralink,

which is admittedly crude and from the outside,

few discussions with people there,

is that they too are going to pursue clinical goals first.

Things like trying to generate smooth movement

in a Parkinsonian patient.

Trying to adjust movement patterns

in someone with Huntington’s disease, for instance.

Things of that sort.

Before they embark on the more sci-fi-like explorations

of 50Xing communication or doubling memory capacity

and these kinds of things.

Although I don’t know,

they may be doing all of those things in parallel.

What are your thoughts about super capabilities of the brain

or super, I don’t even know what word to use.

Supercharging the brain.

Giving the brain functions

for which we’ve never observed before in human history.

But we have our Einsteins and our Feynmans

and our Merzenichs and the, you know,

it’s unclear who to put in along that line, side by side.

But there are some, there are Michael Jordans and et cetera.

But we’ve never heard of or seen somebody

who can jump 20 feet in the air.

Or we’ve heard of people who have photographic memories,

but I don’t know that we are aware

of any human being in history

who could memorize the entire Library of Congress

or all the works within the Vatican within an hour.

Anyway, you get the idea.

What are your thoughts about manipulating neural circuitry

to achieve superhuman or superhuman

or super physiological functions?

Are we there or should we even be thinking about that?

Is it possible, given that neurons simply communicate

through electrical activity and electrical activity

can be engineered outside of the brain?

How do you think about it?

And here we don’t even have to think about Neuralink

in particular, it’s just but one example

of companies and people in laboratories

that are quite understandably considering all this.

Well, it’s a really interesting time right now.

The science has been going on for decades.

The work that we’ve done in this field

that you call brain machine interface,

it’s been going on for a while.

And a lot of the early work was just trying

to restore things like our movement

or having people or monkeys control a computer cursor,

for example, on the screen.

That’s been going on for decades.

What’s been really new is that industry is now involved

and some of this is now becoming commercialized

and we’re starting to see us now cross over to this field

where it’s no longer just research

that we’re talking about medical products

that are designed to be surgically implanted.

In some cases, there’s people doing this kind of work

non-invasively as well that don’t require surgery.

The specific question that you’re asking about

is an area that we call augmentation.

So can you build a device

that essentially enhances someone’s ability

beyond super normal, super memory,

super communication speeds beyond speech, for example?

Or I guess superior precision athletic abilities?

I think that these are very serious kind of questions

to be asking now because as you mentioned,

the pathway so far is really to focus

on these medical applications.

I personally don’t think that we’ve thought enough actually

about what these kind of scenarios are gonna look like.

And I don’t think we’ve thought through

all the ethical implications of what this means

for augmentation in particular.

There’s part of this that is not new at all.

Humans throughout history have been doing things

to augment our function.

Coffee, nicotine, all kinds of things,

all kinds of medications that cross over

from medical to consumer, that is everywhere.

So the pursuit of augmentation or performance

or enhancement is really not a new thing.

The questions really, as they relate to neurotechnologies,

for example, have to do with the invasive nature.

For example, if these technologies require surgery,

for example, to do something

that is not for a medical application.

Again, there, that is not exactly new territory either.

People do that routinely for cosmetic kind of procedures,

for physical appearance, not necessarily cognitive.

So I do think that provided the technology continues

to emerge the way that it does,

that it’s gonna be around the corner.

And it probably is not gonna be in ways

that are super obvious.

I don’t think it’s gonna be like,

can we easily memorize every fact in the world?

But in forms that are gonna be much more incremental

and maybe more subtle, in many ways,

we already have that now.

Like, for example, you don’t have to have

a neural interface embedded in your brain

to get information, essentially access

to all information in the world.

You just have to have your iPhone.

Whether you could do it faster through a brain interface,

I definitely wouldn’t rule that out.

But think about this, that the systems that we have already

to speak and to communicate have evolved

over thousands and millions of years.

And they’re supported by neural structures

that have bandwidth of millions of neurons.

There’s no technology that exists right now

that people are thinking about

that are in commercial form, certainly.

Not even in research labs that come anywhere close

to what has been evolved for those natural purposes.

So I’m essentially saying two sides of this,

which is we’re already getting into this now.

This is not new territory.

This topic of augmentation, both physical and cognitive,

we’ve already surpassed that.

That’s part of what humans do in general.

But we are entering this area of, like,

enhanced cognition, these areas that I think

the technology is gonna be the rate-limiting step

in how far you can go.

We have not had the full conversations about,

number one, is this what we actually want?

Is this gonna be good for society?

Who gets access to this technology?

These are all things that are gonna become

real-world problems.

Certainly a lot to consider.

In thinking about augmentation and another theme

that I’ve yet to ask you about,

but I’m extremely curious about, which is facial expressions.

Before we talk about the relationship

between the musculature of the face and language

and the communication of emotion,

I’d love for you to, if you would,

touch on a little bit of what you’re doing

with patients like Poncho to move beyond

somebody who’s locked in being able to type out

words on a screen with their thoughts.

There’s a rich array of information contained

within the face and facial expression.

And while somebody like Poncho going from having to,

you know, be completely locked in

to being able to peck out letters on a keyboard,

to being able to just think of those letters

and having them spelled out,

that’s a tremendous set of leaps forward towards normalcy.

It’s still far and away different

than Poncho speaking with his mouth,

which I think knowing some people who are restricted,

who are quadriplegic, you know,

a lot of what they struggle with in the real world

is actually a height difference sometimes

because they’re seated while other people are standing.

This actually, we don’t often think about this,

but always have to look up to communicate with people.

It’s a very different interface in the world.

They manage quite well, of course.

But could you tell us what you’re doing

in terms of merging the brain machine interface

with extraction of speech signals

from people who are locked in like Poncho

with facial expressions?

Sure, yeah.

Well, like we described before,

progress is being made.

The proof of principle is out there

that you can decode speech.

That will continue to optimize.

And I’m very confident that that’s gonna improve

very, very quickly in the coming years

to the point where it’s like, you know,

not just a small vocabulary, but a large vocabulary

and at reasonable rates,

at a level that’s gonna be really helpful.

I’m very optimistic about that.

I think it’s the right time to start really thinking about

a broader vision of what communication really is.

So for example, I’m here with you in person.

We could have done this virtually, probably.

It’s pretty easy to do that.

We could have recorded this really separate,

but there is something about being able

to actually see your expressions

and to understand other forms of communication.

So another really important one is nonverbal,

the expressions that you’re making.

You know, for example,

if you have a quizzical look on your face,

if I’m saying something not clear,

that’s a sign to me that I need to rephrase it

or to say it in a different way or to slow down.

For example, or if there’s something

that really excites you,

I want to continue to say more about it

and talk more in detail, you know,

essentially about a given thing.

So facial expressions actually are a really important part

of the way we speak.

And there’s two things.

It’s not just the expressions of like how you’re feeling

and perceiving what I’m saying,

but it’s also seeing my mouth move.

In your eyes, I actually see my mouth move

and my jaw move in a particular way

that actually allows you to hear those sounds better.

So having both the visual information,

but also the sounds go into your brain

is going to improve intelligibly,

also make it more natural.

And memory for what is spoken?

Perhaps.

So here’s a call for people,

not just listening to podcasts,

but watching them and listening to them on YouTube,

I suppose, if we were to sort of translate this

to the real world.

Exactly.

And the reason why we’re also very interested

in this idea of not just having text on a screen,

but essentially a fully computer animated face,

like an avatar of the person’s speech movements

and their facial expressions

is going to be a more complete form of expression.

Now you can imagine right now,

that might just be someone looking at a computer screen,

interpreting these signals.

But I think the way things are going

in the next couple of years,

a lot more of our social interactions,

more than even now,

are going to move into this digital virtual space.

And of course, most people are thinking about

what that means for most consumers,

but it also has really important implications

for people who are disabled, right?

And how are they going to participate in that?

And so we’re thinking really about for people like Pancho

and other people who are paralyzed,

what other forms of BCI can we do

in order to help improve their ability to communicate?

So one is essentially building out more holistic avatars,

you know, things that can essentially decode,

you know, essentially their expressions

or the movements associated with their mouth and jaw

when they actually speak to improve that communication.

So do you envision a time not too long from now

where instead of tweeting out something in text,

my avatar will, I’ll type it out,

but my avatar will just say it.

It’ll be an image of my avatar saying whatever it is

I happen to be tweeting at that moment.

That’s what we’re working on, yeah.

So I don’t think that that is going to happen

and it’s going to happen soon.

And there’s a lot of progress in that.

And again, we’re just trying to enrich

the field of, you know, of communication expression

to make it more normal.

And we actually think that having that kind of avatar

is a way of getting feedback to people

learning how to speak through a speech neuroprosthetic.

That’s the device that we call it.

It’s a speech neuroprosthetic.

That is going to be the way that can help people

learn how to do it the quickest,

not necessarily like trying to say words

and having it come on a screen,

but actually have people embody,

feel like it’s part of themselves

or that they are directly controlling

that illustration or animation.

This idea of an avatar speaking out

what we would otherwise write

is fascinating to me on Instagram.

I post videos.

I don’t filter them,

but I know there’s a lot of discussion nowadays

about people using filters

to make their skin look different

or the lighting look different,

a lot of filtering and also the use of captions.

So that essentially what you end up with

is somewhere between an actual raw video

of what was spoken and an avatar version of it.

I mean, if the mismatch between what’s spoken

and what’s in the caption is too dramatic,

then it doesn’t quite work.

But I watch these carefully when people use captions.

And oftentimes there’s a smoothing

of what was said into the caption.

So it seems much more succinct and accurate.

Oftentimes the reverse is also true

where the caption is inaccurate

and then it creates this kind of jarring mismatch.

In any case, I think this aspect in the clinical realm

of using an avatar to allow people like Poncho

to essentially be a face that communicates

through spoken language from an avatar

that looks like them is fascinating and indeed important.

And I think how avatars emerge in social spaces

is going to be really fascinating.

I get a lot of questions about stutter.

I think that for people who have a stutter,

it is itself anxiety provoking.

Is stutter related to anxiety?

If one has a stutter, what can they do?

Does stutter reflect some underlying neurologic phenomenon

that might distinguish between one kind of stutter

and another?

What can people with stutter do

if they’d like to relieve their stutter?

Yeah, great question.

Stutter is a condition

where the words can’t come out fluently.

So you have all the ideas, you’ve got the language intact.

You remember we talked about this distinction

between language and speech.

Stuttering is a problem of speech, right?

So the ideas, the meanings, the grammar,

it’s all there in people’s stutter,

but they can’t get the words out fluently.

So that’s a speech condition.

And in particular,

it’s a condition that affects articulation,

specifically about controlling the production of words

in this really coordinated kind of movements

that have to happen in the vocal tract

to produce fluent speech.

And stuttering is a condition

where people have a predisposition to it.

So there’s an aspect of stuttering.

You are a stutterer or you’re not a stutterer, right?

But people who stutter don’t stutter all the time either.

So you could be a stutterer who stutters at some times,

but not others.

And really the main link between stuttering and anxiety

is that anxiety can provoke it and make it worse.

That’s certainly true,

but it’s not necessarily caused by anxiety.

It can essentially trigger it or make it worse,

but it’s not the cause of it per se.

So the cause of it is still really not clear,

but it does have to do with these kind of brain functions

that we’ve been talking about earlier,

which is that in order to produce normal fluent speech,

we’re not even conscious of what is going on

in our mouths, in our larynx.

We’re not conscious.

And if we were, we would not be able to speak

because it’s too complex.

It’s too precise.

It’s something that we have really developed

the abilities to do and we do it naturally, right?

It’s part of our programming

and part of what we learn inherently,

and it’s just through exposure.

So stuttering is essentially a breakdown

at certain times in that machinery,

being able to work in a really coordinated way.

You can think about the operations of these areas

that are controlling the vocal tract.

Let’s say speech is like a symphony.

In order for it to come out normally,

you’ve got to have not just one part,

the larynx, but the lips, the jaw.

They can’t be doing their own thing.

They have to be very, very precisely activated

and very, very precisely controlled

in a way to actually create words.

And so in stuttering,

there’s a breakdown of that coordination.

If somebody has a stutter,

is it better to address that early in life

when there’s still neuroplasticity that is very robust?

And if so, what’s the typical route for treatment?

I have to imagine it’s not brain surgery, typically.

I’m guessing there are speech therapists

that people can talk to and they can help them work out

where they’re getting stuck

in their relationship to anxiety.

Yeah, exactly.

I mean, part of it is about that anxiety,

but a lot of it really has to do with therapy

to sort of like work through and think of tricks,

basically, sometimes to create conditions

where you can actually get the words to come out.

A lot of, some forms of stuttering

are really initiation problems.

Just getting started itself is very hard.

You want to start with the initial vowel or consonant,

but it won’t emit.

And so a lot of that therapy is really just focusing

on like how do you create the conditions

for that to happen?

There’s another aspect to it

that I find very interesting is that the feedback,

essentially what we hear ourselves say, for example,

and every time that I say a word,

I’m also hearing what I’m saying.

So that’s what we call auditory feedback.

That turns out to be very important.

And sometimes when you change that,

it can actually change the amount someone stutters

for better or for worse.

And it’s giving us a clue that the brain

is not just focused on sending the commands out,

but it’s also possibly interacting with the part

that is hearing the sounds.

And there’s something might be going on in that connection

that breaks down when stuttering occurs.

So there are individuals that are stutterers,

but they don’t stutter all the time.

In those instances, there’s something happening

in those particular moments

where this very, very precise coordination needs to happen

in the brain in order to get the words out fluently.

We talked a little bit about caffeine and why you avoid it

because your work requires such precision and calm.

And frankly, to me,

it seems like you’re running a lot of operations

and no pun intended in parallel when you’re doing surgery,

not just thinking about where to direct the instruments,

but also thinking like a chess player,

several steps down the line, what could happen?

What if, if then type thinking.

What are some of the other practices and tools

that you use to put yourself into state

for optimal neurosurgery or for, you know,

thinking about scientific problems for that matter.

We keep threatening to go running together,

but I know you run, correct?

Yeah.

Do you find running to be an essential part

of your state regulation?

Absolutely.

Yeah, so for me, most exercise that I do,

I really don’t do for physical reasons.

I do it for mental reasons.

I can tell, for example,

if I don’t go on a run or a swim just after a day or two

and it can have translation, for example,

and the way I feel in the operating room

or even the way I interact with other people.

So there’s no question that those, you know,

the mind and body are deeply connected.

And for me personally,

being able to have opportunity to disconnect for a while,

it turns out to be really, really important.

Now, the operating room for me is another space,

kind of like running or swimming,

where I’m disconnected from the rest of the world.

I don’t bring my cell phone into the operating room.

I’m disconnected from the external world

for that time that I’m in the surgery.

And all I am doing is just focusing.

Now, that doesn’t mean that I’m having complex thoughts

or doing something very complicated.

Sometimes it is like that, but it’s not always like that.

There are things that we do in surgery

that are like routine and rote

and are from muscle memory.

So for example, suturing skin

or doing certain kinds of dissection

or drilling part of the bone, for example,

these are all things that become very rote after a time.

So for me, even being in the operating room

actually can sometimes fulfill that purpose.

So I really look forward to being in the operating room

because that intense focus

allows me to sort of disconnect

from all the other things that I’m worrying about

that are happening on the outside world.

We all have those kinds of things that happen,

and I’m certainly no exception to that.

But strangely, the operating room for me is a sanctuary.

I love being there

because we have some control over the environment.

I know what is there.

I know the anatomy of the brain.

My motions are going through routines.

And so for me, that’s not actually very different

than going on a run and letting my legs

move in specific ways.

It’s just the same thing for my hands.

Do you listen to music or audio books when you run

or are you divorced from technology when you run?

Well, music helps me just stay motivated

and distracted from being out of breath and other things.

And for me, it’s a way just to catch up with the world.

So sometimes I do,

but I do notice that I don’t run as well, for example.

In the operating room, it’s a little different.

Different surgeons have preferences.

I’m more of the camp

where I don’t like any distraction whatsoever.

I’d like people to be able to hear the words

that I’m saying without having background noise.

I don’t really think about relying on music

or other things to try to put me in a state of mind.

I think just being there alone

and just trying to treat it the way it is,

it’s a sacred moment where someone’s life

is really directly under your hands.

That enough kind of focuses me very quickly.

And I like that.

It really detaches me from a lot of things

that are preoccupying me.

And for those couple of hours that we have a surgery,

we’re just focused on one thing only.

It’s fantastic.

Again, I think of in the range of brain explorers,

the neurosurgeons, those of your profession,

are to me like the astronauts of neuroscience

because they’re really going

to the farthest reaches possible

and they’re testing and probing

and really at the front edge of discovering

from the species that we arguably care about the most,

which is humans.

Eddie, I have to say,

from the first time we became friends, 38 years ago.

Something like that.

Something like that.

I’m almost reluctant to say,

but so I only reveal it in part

that Eddie and I became friends

because both he and I shared a love of birds

and we had a club at our school

of which there were only two members, Eddie and I.

Small club.

Small club.

There was one honorary member

and there were certain requirements

for being in this club that we won’t reveal.

We took a pact of secrecy

and we’re going to obey that pact of secrecy.

But to be sitting here with you today

for me is a absolute thrill,

not just because we’ve been friends for that long

or that we got reacquainted

through literally the halls of medicine and science,

but because I really do see what you’re doing

as really representing that front,

absolute cutting edge of exploration and application.

I mean, the story of Poncho is about one

of your many patients

that has derived tremendous benefit from your work.

And now as a chair of a department,

you of course work alongside individuals

who are also doing incredible work

in the spinal cord, et cetera.

So on behalf of myself and everyone listening,

I just really want to thank you

for joining us today to share this information.

We will certainly have you back

because there’s an entire list of other questions

we didn’t have time to get to,

but also just for the work you do.

It’s truly spectacular.

Andrew, thanks so much.

You know, I’m very humbled basically

by what you just said.

And I feel that it’s really an extraordinary honor,

actually, and privileged, you know,

to be here with you and reconnect

and talk about all these ideas.

It’s probably not random, you know,

that we ended up in similar spots and interests.

I think when we were kids, you know,

it starts with some deep interests

and kind of nerding out on topics.

And it’s probably not a coincidence, you know,

that we have such deep interests in this work now.

I just feel really lucky to be able to do what I do.

It’s fun every day, almost every day,

be able to go to work and take care of folks

and learn at the same time,

and then just close the loop.

You know, how do we apply the knowledge

that we learn one day to someone who comes in next week?

It’s really fun.

And we don’t know everything.

We’re not even close to it.

But the journey to figure this out is,

it’s really extraordinary.

I mean, it’s, like you said,

it’s exploring new lands,

literally in the operating room

when I’m looking at the exposed cortex,

trying to understand,

is it safe to walk down this part of the cortical landscape

or this other trail?

You know, which one is gonna be the one

that is gonna be safe versus the other

that results in paralysis and inability to talk?

Well, maybe I shouldn’t call it fun,

but it’s very important too,

in addition to being really intellectually important

for how we understand how the brain works.

And so, yeah,

I feel just really lucky to be in that opportunity.

And we’re lucky to have you being one of the people

doing it, so thank you ever so much.

Thanks.

Thank you for joining me today

for my discussion with Dr. Eddie Chang.

If you’d like to learn more about his research

into the neuroscience of speech and language

and bioengineering, his treatment of epilepsy

and other aspects and diseases and disorders of the brain,

please check out the links in our show note captions.

We have links to his laboratory website,

his clinical website and other resources

related to his critical research as well.

If you’re learning from

and are enjoying the Huberman Lab Podcast,

please subscribe to our YouTube channel.

That’s a terrific zero cost way to support us.

In addition, please subscribe to the Huberman Lab Podcast

on Spotify and Apple.

And on both Spotify and Apple,

you also have the opportunity to leave us

up to a five-star review.

If you have questions for us or comments

about the information we’ve covered

or suggestions about future guests,

please put those in the comment section on YouTube.

We do read all the comments.

Please also check out the sponsors mentioned

at the beginning of today’s episode.

That’s the best way to support the Huberman Lab Podcast.

Not so much today, but in many previous episodes

of the Huberman Lab Podcast, we talk about supplements.

While supplements aren’t necessary for everybody,

many people derive tremendous benefit from them

for things like enhancing sleep and focus

and hormone optimization.

The Huberman Lab Podcast

has partnered with Momentous Supplements.

If you’d like to see the supplements

that the Huberman Lab Podcast has partnered

with Momentous on, you can go to livemomentous,

spelled O-U-S, so livemomentous.com slash Huberman.

And there you’ll see a number of the supplements

that we talk about regularly on the podcast.

I should just mention that that catalog

of supplements is constantly being updated.

As mentioned at the beginning of today’s episode,

the Huberman Lab Podcast has now launched a premium channel.

That premium channel will feature monthly AMAs

or Ask Me Anythings where I answer your questions in depth,

as well as other premium resources.

If you’d like to subscribe to the premium channel,

you can simply go to hubermanlab.com slash premium.

I should mention that the proceeds from the premium channel

go to support the standard Huberman Lab Podcast,

which will continue to be released every Monday per usual,

as well as supporting various research projects

done on humans to create the sorts of tools

for mental health, physical health, and performance

that you hear about on the Huberman Lab Podcast.

Again, it’s hubermanlab.com slash premium to subscribe.

It’s $10 a month or $100 per year.

If you haven’t already subscribed

to our zero cost newsletter,

we have what is called the Neural Network Newsletter.

You can subscribe by going to hubermanlab.com,

go to the menu and click on newsletter.

Those newsletters include summaries of podcast episodes,

lists of tools from the Huberman Lab Podcast.

And if you’d like to see previous newsletters

we’ve released, you can also just go to hubermanlab.com,

click on newsletter in the menu,

and you’ll see various downloadable PDFs.

If you want to sign up for the newsletter,

we just ask for your email.

We do not share your email with anybody.

And again, it’s completely zero cost.

If you’re not already following me on social media,

it’s Huberman Lab on Twitter, on Facebook,

and on Instagram.

And at all three of those places,

I cover topics and subject matter

that are sometimes overlapping with the information

covered on the Huberman Lab Podcast,

but that’s often distinct from information

on the Huberman Lab Podcast.

Again, it’s Huberman Lab on all social media channels.

Thank you once again for joining me today

for the discussion about the neuroscience of speech,

language, epilepsy, and much more with Dr. Eddie Chang.

And as always, thank you for your interest in science.

comments powered by Disqus