Welcome to the Huberman Lab Podcast,
where we discuss science
and science-based tools for everyday life.
I’m Andrew Huberman,
and I’m a professor of neurobiology and ophthalmology
at Stanford School of Medicine.
Today, we are discussing creativity.
Creativity is a topic that to many people is very abstract.
That is, we know when something seems creative.
Some of us know people who are creative
or perhaps are creative,
and yet the ability to be creative resides in everybody.
And we know that because the neural circuits
that underlie creativity have been somewhat defined,
and the steps and processes within the brain and body
that lead to creativity are well-known.
That said, most people don’t know how to access creativity,
and if they do know how to access creativity,
they are only able to access creativity
in a fairly limited number of domains of life.
For instance, in the visual arts, or in music,
or within science, or engineering,
or any number of different domains
ranging from the kitchen to sport
to childhood interactions, that is, childhood games.
In other words, some adults are able
to access their creative spirit
when engaging in childlike play with children,
or for that matter, with adults.
But as it turns out, all of creativity stems
from just a small subset of neural structures in the brain
that need to be activated in a particular sequence or order.
Today, we will talk about what those neural structures are,
what particular order they need to be activated in
in order to come up with, for instance,
new ideas that are creative,
and then how to implement those creative strategies.
We will also talk about different ways to access creativity
that include narrative and storytelling,
as well as applying new rule sets
or even entirely new worldviews.
And we will do this in a structured way
that will allow anyone,
whether or not you consider yourself creative or not,
to be able to apply these tools
in different domains of life,
work, family, play, and on and on.
By the end of today’s episode,
you will have a better understanding
of what creativity is and how to access it,
and if you like, to bring others
into your creative endeavors,
which, as you’ll soon learn,
can massively expand the extent to which you yourself
can express your creative talents,
as is the case with all episodes
of the Huberman Lab Podcast.
Today, we will discuss both scientific mechanisms
and nomenclature,
and I promise to make all of that clear to you,
even if you don’t have a background in biology or psychology,
but we will also, of course, discuss tools,
that is specific steps that you can take
in order to be more creative.
One particular tool that I’m excited to share with you
involves a meditation,
but this is a very unusual meditation.
This is not sitting with eyes closed,
focusing on your breath,
or focusing on a chime,
or some other feature in your sensory environment,
or even in your body.
Later, we will talk about open monitoring meditations.
Open monitoring meditations are very distinct
from other forms of meditation,
and involve learning how to sit back
and simply observe your thoughts
while intentionally varying where your thoughts go.
So for those of you that find it a struggle
to focus or to refocus
in more traditional forms of meditation,
or maybe even in your work,
and even for those of you that may suffer
from things like ADHD or similar,
open monitoring meditation can be an extremely valuable tool
for accessing your creative abilities
because of the ways that it allows you
to tap into specific circuits
within the frontal networks of your brain.
So these are networks of the brain
that include the areas just behind your forehead,
and that allow you to evaluate new and novel rule sets
in a very unconstrained way.
Because if you think about it,
creativity is really the ability
to take existing elements from the physical world,
or from the thought world, if you will,
or from any domain of life,
mood, thinking, and information,
and to reorder those into novel combinations
that are useful for something.
And as we’ll also find out later,
creativity has this incredible aspect to it,
which is that when we see or create
or experience something that is truly creative,
it reveals to us something fundamental
about the way that the natural world,
and indeed the way that our brains work.
If that sounds very mysterious and abstract to you now,
I promise that by the end of today’s episode,
you will not only understand what that means,
but you will also understand
how to use open monitoring meditations,
as well as other forms of tools
in order to access your creative ability.
Before we begin, I’d like to emphasize
that this podcast is separate from my teaching
and research roles at Stanford.
It is, however, part of my desire and effort
to bring zero cost to consumer information about science
and science-related tools to the general public.
In keeping with that theme,
I’d like to thank the sponsors of today’s podcast.
Our first sponsor is Roka.
Roka makes eyeglasses and sunglasses
that are the absolute highest quality.
The company was founded by two all-American swimmers
from Stanford, and everything about Roka eyeglasses
and sunglasses were designed with performance in mind.
I’ve spent a lifetime working on the biology
of the visual system, and I can tell you
that your visual system has to contend
with an enormous number of challenges
for you to be able to see clearly.
For instance, when you move from a shady area
to a sunny area, there are all sorts of adaptations
that have to occur in your eye and brain.
Roka understands these kinds of adaptations,
and the other ones required to see clearly,
and those are built into the engineering
of their sunglasses and eyeglasses.
One of the things that makes Roka eyeglasses
and sunglasses so terrific
is that they are extremely lightweight.
In fact, most of the time, I don’t even realize
that they’re on my face.
I wear sunglasses when I drive
and whenever I need to throughout the day,
and I wear readers at night.
I do not wear sunglasses when I get my morning sunlight,
which I do every single morning,
as you should be doing also,
as is covered many times on this podcast.
Roka eyeglasses and sunglasses are also unique
in that unlike a lot of so-called performance eyeglasses
out there, they don’t make you look like a cyborg.
They have some of the styles
that make you look like a cyborg, if that’s your thing,
but if you want to wear glasses or sunglasses out to dinner
or around for social reasons,
they have many different aesthetic styles to choose from.
If you’d like to try Roka eyeglasses or sunglasses,
go to roka.com, that’s R-O-K-A.com,
and enter the code Huberman to save 20% off your order.
Again, that’s Roka, R-O-K-A.com,
and enter the code Huberman at checkout.
Today’s episode is also brought to us by Thesis.
Thesis makes custom nootropics.
And as you may have heard me say before,
I am not a fan of the word nootropics
because it means smart drugs.
And frankly, there is no neural circuit in the brain
for being quote-unquote smart.
There are neural circuits for focus.
There are neural circuits for task switching.
There are neural circuits for today’s topic,
which is creativity.
Nootropics, therefore, is not a great word
to describe any sort of supplement or drug
that can enhance brain function
because it lacks specificity.
Thesis understands this,
and therefore has designed custom nootropics
that are tailored to your specific needs
and that allow you to enter the brain and bodily states
that are optimal for things like focus,
energy, creativity, and so on.
If you’d like to try
your own personalized nootropic starter kit,
go online to takethesis.com slash Huberman,
take their brief three-minute quiz,
and Thesis will send you four different formulas
to try in your first month.
Again, that’s takethesis.com slash Huberman,
and use the code Huberman at checkout
for 10% off your first box.
Today’s episode is also brought to us by Element.
Element is an electrolyte drink
that contains everything you need,
that is sodium, magnesium, and potassium
in the precise ratios that you need without sugar.
As I’ve talked about many times before on this podcast
and elsewhere, every cell in your body
requires hydration and electrolytes
in order to function properly.
And a key example of this are the neurons,
the nerve cells of your brain
that allow you to think clearly,
to exercise hard, et cetera.
Neurons require sodium
in order to fire what are called action potentials
or electrical signaling between neurons,
and they require potassium and magnesium,
and those have to be present in the correct ratios
in order for your brain and body to function optimally.
Element is formulated to help anyone
with their electrolyte needs
and is perfectly suited to anyone following
a ketogenic, low-carbohydrate, or paleolithic diet,
or a basic omnivore diet, which is the one that I follow.
If you’d like to try Element,
you can go to drinkelement, that’s lmnt.com slash Huberman
to claim a free Element sample pack with your purchase.
And right now, Element has two special flavors
for the holidays, chocolate caramel and mint chocolate,
which I should mention tastes especially good
if you actually heat it up.
So if you treat it sort of like a tea, it works,
or if you drink it cold, it’s delicious.
All the flavors of Element I find delicious.
Again, you can go to drinkelement,
that’s lmnt.com slash Huberman.
The Huberman Lab Podcast
is now partnered with Momentous Supplements.
To find the supplements we discuss
on the Huberman Lab Podcast,
you can go to livemomentous, spelled O-U-S,
livemomentous.com slash Huberman.
And I should just mention that the library
of those supplements is constantly expanding.
Again, that’s livemomentous.com slash Huberman.
Let’s talk about creativity.
Now on the face of it, the word creativity
and creative acts might seem somewhat abstract to us.
That is, we know when we see something
that we consider creative,
and we know when we see something that is not creative,
things that aren’t creative are things
that we see every day.
A car with four tires, for instance,
a bicycle with two tires, not creative.
However, we also see things that are novel,
that are different,
and that we don’t really think of as creative.
In fact, they can be downright trivial.
For instance, if I were to take a fish tank
and put wings on it,
that’s a novel combination of things,
which is one of the key criteria for an act
or an object or a piece of music that is creative.
And yet, neither of us, I believe,
would find it very creative or very interesting
that a fish tank has wings on it.
Why, or why not, I should say?
Well, it turns out that for something to be creative,
it actually has to reveal to us something fundamental
about the world or about how we work.
And I must say that oftentimes,
the most creative and the most interesting
and the most beloved creative acts
reveal to us something fundamental about the world
or the way that we work,
in a way that delights and thrills and surprises us,
but that we aren’t even aware
what that fundamental rule is.
I’ll return to this in a few minutes,
but the time being,
let’s just build up from first principles.
What constitutes something creative
and what does not constitute something creative?
Creativity is a way of interacting with the world
or combining or recombining things in the world
in a way that appears novel to us and to other people.
My example of a fish tank with wings on it is novel,
but frankly, it’s not very creative
and it’s not very interesting.
It doesn’t reveal anything new to us.
Sure, they’re flying fish,
although they just kind of jump far, they don’t really fly.
And as a consequence, putting wings on a fish tank
could be used as a metaphor
for the fact that fish don’t fly,
but you already knew and I already knew that fish don’t fly.
And so there’s nothing novel revealed to us about the world
except something we already knew.
Now, creative acts on the other hand,
of course involve novel combinations of existing rule sets.
That could be different combinations of music
or colors or shapes or technology, et cetera.
But it does so in a way
that tells us something fundamental and different.
Let me give an example
of a few truly creative artistic acts.
And I’ll do that in the domain of visual arts,
but of course there are many examples
that could come from music or from other domains,
sport, et cetera.
Examples I’ll give rather than a fish tank with wings
are for instance, the comparison between a drawing
or a very accurate painting of a face,
an Escher painting and a Banksy.
Okay, if you don’t know what those are, I’ll explain.
First of all, let’s talk about
an accurate representation of a face.
If I were to sit you down
or if you were to send me a photograph
and then I were to paint or draw a picture of your face
in a way that faithfully represented the position
and shape of your nose relative to the eyes,
maybe a curl of the lip,
maybe a few hairs of your eyebrows in a particular way
that really captured you accurately.
I think most people would say, okay, it’s accurate.
It looks a lot like the photograph or the person.
And on the one hand, while that could be interesting,
it’s not particularly creative
because it faithfully represents what’s already there.
In contrast, a painting or a picture like an Escher,
and for those of you that aren’t familiar with Eschers,
involves a lot of repeating patterns.
So for instance, a bird image that’s repeated
over and over and over and over again,
sometimes in partially overlapping manner,
and perhaps a building that’s repeated
over and over and over again,
or stones repeated over and over again,
or staircases over and over again.
Eschers capture elements from the outside world
and faithfully represent them,
but faithfully represent them over and over and over again,
which is not typically seen in the natural world.
In fact, most of what our visual system does
is to eliminate repetitive patterns when we see them.
In fact, most of what our visual system does
is try and make us blind to repetitive patterns
in our visual environment,
and only allow us to see things that are unusual
in that visual environment.
Now, this is especially true at visual scales.
What I mean by that is if you were to go to the beach
and lie on your towel and look down at the sand,
you would start to notice
that the sand is a very, very repetitive pattern.
So at very small scales,
and in particular at molecular scales,
when you get down to the level of atoms and so forth,
everything is repetitive.
It’s the same thing,
it’s just reproduced in different combinations
over and over again.
But as we move through our world,
typically we’re not looking down at pebbles on the ground
or little grains of sand,
or the pattern of leaves in a particular clover
or something of that sort.
Most of the time we’re looking out on landscapes
or at people’s faces, et cetera.
And very seldom do we see
highly repetitive patterns at that scale.
So what Eschers do is they essentially reveal to us
a fundamental feature
about the way that our visual system works,
which is that repetitive patterns
tend to become noise in our visual system.
That is our brain encodes repetition
as things not to be interested in.
And the things that stand out against that repetition
as the things to be interested in,
so-called signal to noise.
What Eschers do is they invert the relationship
between signal and noise,
and they make the repetitive patterns, the signal,
and the unusual patterns, the noise.
In fact, in every Escher there are unusual patterns
and those completely disappear to us.
Now, when you look at an Escher,
what you probably see and what I see
are just a bunch of birds repeated over and over again,
or buildings or staircases repeated over and over again.
And you may like Eschers and you may not,
that’s not the point.
Today, we’re not talking about taste
in particular creative acts.
What we’re trying to identify here
are the rules and mechanisms
of what constitutes something creative
and why it’s creative.
And the key element here is that what’s revealed by an Escher
through these repetition patterns
is an inversion of the way
that our brain normally encodes visual images.
And therefore the rule that repetition
is suppressed in our visual system
and that unusual visual features are revealed to us,
that rule is what pops out to us
when we look at an Escher.
Now, when I say pops out,
I don’t mean that you look at an Escher and go,
oh, normally I don’t see repetition,
normally I see the unusual stuff, et cetera, et cetera.
But there seems to be something about truly creative acts
that capture the attention
and sometimes the delight of many, many people.
It’s that they reveal a fundamental rule
about how the brain or the world work.
Let me give you a different example,
also from the visual art world.
Let me give you the example of Banksy.
Banksy is an artist
that many of you are probably familiar with
and probably some of you are not familiar with.
So for those of you that are not familiar with Banksy,
Banksy is an artist
that most often does two-dimensional artwork.
So these would be stencils or paintings or drawings,
like many artists,
and does them in an urban landscape,
an actual city or suburban landscape.
That is, he draws our stencils or graffitis
in a very cryptic way, I should say,
no one really knows who Banksy is or when he does his art,
he just reveals his art by putting it up,
but he does this in the context of cities
and on three-dimensional objects.
So a good example would be,
he will stencil next to a phone booth, a police officer,
or he will graffiti next to an actual fire hydrant,
a dog lifting its leg to urinate on that fire hydrant.
Now, what’s interesting about Banksy’s
is not simply the fact that he puts two-dimensional art
onto three-dimensional surfaces
in the urban and suburban landscape,
because if you think about it,
that’s been done many, many times before,
all graffiti is that, all city art and murals is that.
So what’s unique about Banksy?
What’s unique about Banksy,
or I should say Banksy’s, the actual art,
is that he combines two-dimensional art
with a three-dimensional landscape
in a way that the concept pops out at you.
What do I mean by that?
Well, in the case of the dog lifting its leg to urinate
on the fire hydrant,
that’s a scene that most people,
and in fact, most children are familiar with
from cartoons or from our basic understanding
of the stereotype of dogs,
and I must tell you, having owned a male dog,
a bulldog, Costello, for many years,
hydrants were a particular target for Costello.
Of course, everything was a particular target
for Costello urinating outdoors.
Nonetheless, he liked to pee on fire hydrants.
That itself is not interesting.
Seeing a photograph of a dog raising its leg
to pee on a fire hydrant is not interesting.
Seeing a painting of that isn’t interesting.
Seeing an actual dog urinating on a fire hydrant
isn’t interesting.
In fact, seeing a painting in two dimensions
of a dog raising its leg to,
of course, it can’t actually urinate,
but give you the impression that it would urinate
on that fire hydrant isn’t particularly interesting
except for the fact that it reveals to us
something fundamental,
which is that we tend to pair visual relationships
between different objects that share a common theme,
and then the theme tends to pop out at us.
So for instance, the dog raising its leg
next to a fire hydrant,
even if the dog is drawn in two dimensions
and the fire hydrant is in three dimensions,
allows the concept of dog and fire hydrant
to emerge or pop out at us,
which reveals to us something fundamental
about how our brain works,
which is that our brain encodes concepts and entire stories
as symbols of interaction between different objects.
Let me give you a different example
just to make sure that this hits home.
One of Banksy’s more famous paintings
is a rather politically charged one,
which is of a girl holding a bouquet of balloons,
and this two-dimensional drawing was put onto the West wall,
dividing territories in the Middle East,
a very controversial issue.
The controversies of that issue
are not what I want to get into,
but I don’t think anyone would doubt
that it is a controversial issue.
The two-dimensional drawing of the girl with the balloons
on the actual wall turns out to be quite interesting
as an art piece, because what it reveals to us
is the entire controversy around the presence of that wall
and the desire for certain people to breach that wall
and the desire for other people to insist
that that will not be breached for whatever reason.
Again, this is not about the particular controversy.
The point is that a two-dimensional image
combined with a three-dimensional structure
allows the purpose of that three-dimensional structure
and the controversy around that three-dimensional structure
to pop out at us in a way that if,
for instance, we had just seen a photograph
of somebody next to that wall or with a ladder,
or if we just seen a drawing of a girl
holding a bouquet of balloons on a drawing of that wall
to not emerge.
In other words, it captures two fundamental features
of the visual system, our ability to encode things
in two dimensions and understand symbols,
and our ability to understand things in three dimensions,
and in particular, the wall as a three-dimensional object
is really interesting
because it’s an actual physical barrier.
So showing it as the actual physical barrier
that it is in real space in three dimensions
turns out to allow the interaction between those two things,
the concept, the controversy to pop out at us
and make us think about that particular controversy
and perhaps where we each individually stand
on that controversy.
Now, there are many examples
of what I just gave in the visual domain.
For instance, Rothko’s, which are just color on canvas
are particularly interesting source of information
about the way that the brain encodes color.
Later, I’ll fill in exactly what that information is.
You may like Rothko’s, you may not,
but I’ll tell you one thing.
When you look at a Rothko,
you are seeing colors in a very different way
than you would ever see colors in any other context.
The fact that they don’t have a frame typically,
and the fact that there’s no white canvas
allows the colors that you see to be novel hues
of those colors that you will not see in any other context.
And in doing so reveals to you
what your brain does in order to understand
and extract color.
Now, in the context of music, for instance,
you will sometimes hear a street musician play a song,
maybe a Bob Dylan song or a Led Zeppelin song
or a Pink Floyd song pretty closely, pretty accurately
to the way that song is played.
But of course that’s not creative.
That’s just like the photograph
or the accurate portrait of somebody’s face.
Or you may hear an acoustic version
of what’s normally an electric guitar song
or electrical song or vice versa.
Somewhat creative, sometimes sounds even better
than the original, but not particularly creative.
However, each and every one of us
has a particular taste in music.
Maybe it’s classical, maybe it’s rock,
maybe it’s punk, maybe it’s hip hop.
Within each of those genres,
I think all of us are familiar with hearing something
for the first time and maybe even every time.
And there’s something about the combination
of the words and the music,
or sometimes just the music or just the words
that allows some feature of it to pop out at us
as particularly exciting.
And when we feel that excitement
and we feel that it’s really novel,
it’s different than what we’ve heard before,
I assure you what it’s revealing to you
is the way that your auditory system
and often your auditory and your emotional system
encodes information that you hear.
And again, the rule that it’s revealing
is not splayed out for you.
For instance, it’s not told to you,
oh, this is the way you normally hear
and now you’re hearing things differently.
Sometimes it’s the change in, for instance,
in the way that words are accented
or the way that sentences are constructed.
This often you’ll hear in hip hop,
the way that sentences are constructed
can be divided up, not as normal declarative sentences,
the way that they’re typically written,
but the way that sentences are chopped up and fractured
reveals to us new meaning,
and in fact, enhanced meaning about particular words
that we wouldn’t see if it was written out as a paragraph
and then sung as a script
that would be the same as the one that we would read.
Again, the point is that what is exciting and novel to you
is just the way that you hear it,
but it’s exciting and novel to you
because there are circuits within the brain
that when we hear or see or feel or experience
known elements in new ways that are truly creative,
the way that those neural circuits function is changed.
And when neural circuits change the way that they function
simply by way of what comes into our eyes, our ears,
and the way that we experience our feelings,
there’s the release of chemicals,
including the release of the chemical dopamine
and other neuromodulators as well
that make us feel both surprised, delighted,
and this is very key,
excited in anticipation that we might see it again.
So with the understanding in mind that true creativity
involves the novel combination of some elements,
could be notes of music, could be numbers,
could be visual elements like lines or colors,
could be physical movements, et cetera,
but novel combinations of some things
that reveal to us something fundamental
about the way that our brain and or the world work.
And of course, as I mentioned before,
that fundamental thing may or may not
be consciously accessible to us.
We may not know what exactly it is that’s novel to us,
but it feels novel and it feels true.
Well, with that understanding in mind,
we therefore can ask what are the underlying principles
and neural circuits that underlie the creative process?
And the word process here is especially important.
In fact, if there’s one thing
I’d really like to impress on everybody
is that when thinking about biology,
it’s almost always better to think about verbs
as opposed to nouns.
So rather than think of creativity as a noun
or somebody being creative as an adjective,
think about the verb creativity.
That is, what are the steps required?
And therefore, what are the cells and circuits
and thoughts, et cetera, required in order to be creative?
This element of thinking about verbs
then allows us to say, okay,
what are the various steps in coming up
with a creative idea, in testing a creative idea,
and then implementing that creative idea?
And in doing so, we find,
based on the scientific literature,
that there are basically three major networks
within the brain, each of which is responsible
for each of the three steps
to arrive at something truly creative.
The first neural circuit involved in creativity
is the so-called executive network.
This is kind of a goofy name
because the neural circuits that I’m about to describe
do a bunch of other things as well,
but they certainly control
what are called executive functions.
Executive functions are functions that you and I both have,
which is our ability to govern our thinking
and our behavior in very deliberate ways.
And that is largely accomplished
through the use of the neural circuitry
that sits right behind the forebrain,
the so-called prefrontal cortex.
Now, the prefrontal cortex involves
many different sub-regions.
It has a bunch of different parts,
just like any country has different states, et cetera,
and provinces.
Executive function involves the prefrontal cortex
and some other neural structures.
But for sake of this discussion,
executive function and the prefrontal cortex
are mainly responsible for suppressing action,
that is for eliminating choices
among the infinite number of choices that exist,
for instance, of what colors to combine on a painting
or what lines to draw or what notes to play
or what movements to make in a sports endeavor,
what numbers to include in a mathematics endeavor,
or what words and letters and syllables and sentences
to include in writing a creative passage.
The second network is the so-called default mode network.
There’s a lot of discussion nowadays
about the default mode network
as it relates to consciousness and meditation, et cetera.
The default mode network does many different things,
but in the context of our discussion about creativity,
the default mode network is really the network
that starts being engaged when you close your eyes
and start paying attention to what’s going on
in terms of your thinking
as opposed to the sensory outside world.
And the default mode network is especially important
for what’s called spontaneous imagination.
Now, spontaneous imagination is something
that you can try at any moment
if you were to close your eyes
and to try and not pay attention to the sounds around you,
but even if you do,
to just pay attention to whatever thoughts
or feelings emerge when your eyes are closed, okay?
By closing your eyes and shutting yourself off
to the outside sensory world,
you start to engage much more of your brain machinery
dedicated towards what’s going on inside you,
so-called interoception,
but also what you’re thinking about your thinking,
whether or not your thoughts are complete or incomplete,
whether or not they are fragmentary in a way
that goes from one thought to another,
distantly in the past or present or future, et cetera.
Depending on time of day, how well-rested you are,
how stressed you are, how happy you are,
the default mode network will take you through a journey
of different types of thoughts,
different types of feelings, et cetera.
The specific types of thoughts and feelings
are not as interesting as the fact
that when you close your eyes,
you’re essentially engaging this default mode network,
which is essentially the network associated
with imagination and imagination based on elements
that exist only within your head,
that is within your brain, okay?
And therefore must rely on memory of previous experiences.
As soon as you close your eyes,
you are shutting yourself off from the sensory world.
So by definition, you can no longer be bringing
in novel experiences in that moment.
You’re relying on your library of existing experiences
and your memory of those in order to imagine new things.
And you’re doing this in a very, in a free associative way.
You’re not trying to imagine new things.
It’s just whatever geysers to the surface, okay?
So we’ve got the executive network,
which is involved in suppressing particular thoughts
or actions.
We have the default mode network,
which is involved in imagination.
And the default mode network I should mention
also involves a sub-region of the prefrontal cortex.
It’s called the medial prefrontal cortex,
but other brain regions as well.
And then the final element within the circuits
underlying creativity is the so-called salience network.
The salience network is a network of brain regions
that involves areas such as the insula,
which actually has a complete map of your body surface,
as well as some information mapped there
about what’s going on in the outside world
and how those combine with what’s going on
in your internal landscape that is within your body.
Also a brain region called the ACC or, excuse me,
anterior cingulate cortex, and the amygdala.
So a lot of information is mapped
within the salience network about how we feel
and how we feel in relation to things
that are happening around us and within us.
And the salience network has one main job,
which is to pay attention to what’s most interesting
either in the world or inside us
in terms of feelings or experiences, okay?
So we’ve got three networks, executive network,
which is there to suppress choices
in terms of actions we could take, but decide not to,
or things we could think about,
but choose not to or try not to.
The default mode network,
which is basically the catalog or library
of previous experiences that we have available to us
that would act as sort of the paints on a palette
or the possible ingredients that could go into a recipe.
All of that has to, again,
arise from previous experience, right?
We can’t close our eyes
and suddenly be able to access all the melodies
that we’ve never heard before,
or all our ideas and concepts and knowledge about music
if we don’t have musical understanding
or visual understanding.
So we’re really drawing up the library
and that library tends to be rather disorganized.
It kind of swirls around.
It’s not very structured
unless we’re actively trying to think about something.
And then we have the salience network,
which is the networks within the brain that decide
or make choices about what’s most interesting
to pay attention to in a given moment.
Okay, so those three networks work together
to create things.
And when I say create things,
we again have to really underscore
our definition of creativity.
Creativity is a rearrangement of existing elements
into novel combinations that reveal something fundamental
about how we or the world works.
And this is very important,
it tends to be things that are useful.
Now they can merely be useful
because they’re entertaining or thrilling.
They can also have a particular utility
or use in the world like a piece of technology
that is actually useful,
like an app or a smartphone or a computer
actually has utility or a vehicle.
You know, there are creative acts
that led to the formation of vehicles and computers,
et cetera.
But the point is that just merely coming up
with novel combinations of things
like wings on a fish tank,
that’s not creative or it’s not creative
in any kind of meaningful way
because it’s simply not useful.
It doesn’t reveal anything fundamental,
new or purposeful.
It doesn’t allow us to think about
or interact with the world or ourselves in novel ways.
Whereas things, people, actions
and ideas that are truly creative
really change the way that we are able to access the world.
They act as portals to the world and to ourselves.
I’d like to take a quick break
and acknowledge one of our sponsors, Athletic Greens.
Athletic Greens now called AG1
is a vitamin mineral probiotic drink
that covers all of your foundational nutritional needs.
I’ve been taking Athletic Greens since 2012.
So I’m delighted that they’re sponsoring the podcast.
The reason I started taking Athletic Greens
and the reason I still take Athletic Greens
once or usually twice a day
is that it gets me the probiotics that I need for gut health.
Our gut is very important.
It’s populated by gut microbiota
that communicate with the brain, the immune system
and basically all the biological systems of our body
to strongly impact our immediate and long-term health.
And those probiotics in Athletic Greens are optimal
and vital for microbiotic health.
In addition, Athletic Greens contains a number of adaptogens
vitamins and minerals that make sure
that all of my foundational nutritional needs are met.
And it tastes great.
If you’d like to try Athletic Greens
you can go to athleticgreens.com slash Huberman
and they’ll give you five free travel packs
that make it really easy to mix up Athletic Greens
while you’re on the road, in the car, on the plane, et cetera
and they’ll give you a year supply of vitamin D3K2.
Again, that’s athleticgreens.com slash Huberman
to get the five free travel packs
and the year supply of vitamin D3K2.
So now you have some idea about the brain areas
and networks involved in creativity
but I want to be very clear
that anytime we talk about mechanisms and brain areas
what’s far more important than the names
of those brain areas is an understanding of what they do.
So if you couldn’t remember the anterior cingulate cortex
or the fact that the prefrontal cortex is involved
in executive function, et cetera, that’s fine.
It’s less important that you know the names of things
than you understand the action steps that those things take.
That is the verb actions
that those particular brain areas engage
in order to arrive at a particular end point.
And the end point we’re talking about today is creativity.
I want to discuss creativity in terms
of what actually goes into being creative.
And it turns out there are just two elements
and those two elements are now well-understood
from the perspective of psychology.
And fortunately the neuroscience well supports
what the psychology says and vice versa.
And those two elements that go into coming up
with a creative idea and then implementing
or developing that creative idea into something real
that you and the rest of the world can experience
are divergent thinking and convergent thinking.
And divergent thinking and convergent thinking
are very straightforward to understand.
Divergent thinking is taking some known object
or event in the world or sport or concept.
It could be running, it could be a musical note,
it could be jumping, it could be a particular color
on a piece of paper and asking yourself
how many different things could that thing actually be?
You might say, well, running is running
but let’s use divergent thinking as a way to illustrate
what divergent thinking is.
If I show you a picture of somebody running,
I say, what do you see?
And you say, I see somebody running.
And then I might give you a divergent thinking task
and these tasks are the same ones used
in various experiments.
And I’d say, how many different things can you think about
based on this picture that you see of somebody running?
Now, if you are able to engage divergent thinking,
you could say running to the store,
running away from a lion, running towards somebody I love
or maybe you have a more elaborate imagination
and you could say running in front of a bus to grab a kid
so the kid doesn’t get hit by the bus
or running toward a concert
because I’m so excited about the particular concert
and then it starts to spool into a story.
In other words, divergent thinking involves taking
one simple, what we would call in neuroscience
or psychology, stimulus, one image or sound, et cetera,
and trying to radiate out from that
as many different divergent situations, properties,
characteristics, events, things
from that one specific element.
So any divergent thinking task would involve exactly that.
I’d show you pictures or play you sounds or words or notes
or describe to you events in history
and try and see how many things can radiate out from that
into diverse, diverse, even distant types of concepts
and pictures, okay?
So that’s divergent thinking.
Divergent thinking is really the process
that underlies idea generation
and the basis of divergent thinking
is that more than one idea is correct.
In fact, the more ideas that you have about one thing,
the better your divergent thinking.
So if I were to give you three minutes
to list off all the things you can think about
related to this pen that I’m holding up,
for those of you listening,
I’m just holding up a pen in front of me,
and you just write them out or say them out
over the next three minutes,
that would be an example of divergent thinking.
However, if you just said black pen, red pen, white pen,
green pen, et cetera, that’s not very divergent thinking.
It’s only divergent in the context of color space.
When I say space, that’s just a kind of nerd speak
for one particular domain of thinking.
Whereas if you said red pen, white pen,
essay, pen in a door to hold the door open
so that someone can return to a building
and you started spooling off a story related to that
and why that was important, well, there you go.
Divergent thinking is essentially taking one element
and coming up with many, many answers.
And in the context of divergent thinking, any answer goes,
but as we’ll soon learn,
not every answer is interesting and relevant.
That is not every answer helps solve something
or reveal something fundamental.
And therefore not every divergent answer is truly creative.
The other aspect of divergent thinking
that’s really important to understand
is that the selection criteria are extremely vague and vast.
That is, there are no constraints on what you come up with.
So if I hold up this pen and you say orangutan,
that’s a perfectly valid divergent idea from this pen
because you thought of it and it’s distantly related.
However, we have to remember our earlier rule.
If black pen and orangutan are not linked up in our brain,
in the observer’s brain, in any kind of meaningful way,
it’s only interesting to you
because you are the only one that understands the rule
that underlies the link between this pen and orangutan.
Whereas if you come up with something different
that somehow tells me and everybody else
something interesting about pens or orangutans,
now that’s a truly creative idea.
I don’t have such an example in mind,
but later I’ll give you some examples
of how you can actually march down
the path of divergent thinking
and use that executive network to suppress certain options,
to cross off certain answers.
Because again, any answer’s valid,
but not all valid answers are interesting or useful.
And you can cross those off and arrive at the most
interesting and truly creative answer.
A couple more things about divergent thinking.
Divergent thinking largely taps
into the networks of the brain
that are involved in mental flexibility.
So this is a different aspect of our prefrontal cortex
which is not based on executive function
and our ability to reduce options,
but rather areas of the prefrontal cortex
that are available to generate multiple options
and actually suppress context, right?
To forget that pens are just for writing, for instance,
and that pens can do other things like hold the door open.
It’s really kind of an unusual use of a pen.
Again, none of these examples that I’m giving
are particularly interesting.
They’re just designed to get you to understand
the underlying concept of divergent thinking.
And then the last thing I’d like you to know
about divergent thinking is that divergent thinking
involves a sort of exploration.
It’s a wandering through of ideas that you already had
in your library, in your memory banks about pens
and what pens could be related to
and what pens ought not to be related to.
So again, what’s really important about creativity
is that there has to be the basic building blocks
already existing within us.
This is why it’s so important to understand
that if you are somebody who really seeks to be creative,
you really do need to be somebody who forages
for information and structured information in particular,
if you are to be creative.
The architect simply can’t come up with incredible drawings
or plans for buildings without understanding
how buildings are put together
and the various rules that govern buildings.
In other words, you can’t break rules
that you don’t understand.
I think in movies especially, we have this idea in mind
that of this limitless concept
or that we have these hidden geniuses
that somehow have access to all the math knowledge
without ever having done any formal math.
Actually, I was flying back from Texas recently
and Goodwill Hunting was on somebody’s screen.
I don’t tend to watch movies on the plane very often,
sometimes, but not often.
And I was remembering in that movie,
you’ve got this math genius who is a janitor at MIT,
and apparently just has access to all this knowledge.
It’s a wonderful concept, a very, very,
I would say even exceedingly rare thing
to occur in the world.
Sure, there are people who seem to have a natural talent
for mathematics or for something else,
but this idea that there are incredible geniuses among us
that just spontaneously have so much knowledge,
that’s by far the exception rather than the rule, of course,
and may not even actually exist.
I’m sure someone will put in the comments examples
where this actually exists.
More often than not, what you find is that people
who have extreme virtuosity in a given area
put many, many years into developing the basic substrates,
the basic building blocks of whatever it is
their craft happens to be
where they demonstrate virtuosity.
So this is very important to understand.
Nonetheless, divergent thinking is the critical element
for initiating the creative process.
Again, thinking about creativity as a verb.
And divergent thinking involves taking some starting point,
in this case, a pen, and then radiating out from that
in a fairly unconstrained,
what biologists call a random walk,
just kind of wandering through your thought space
and memory space about what could be related to this pen.
Now, on the flip side of creativity
is the implementation of specific combinations of things
and testing those to see whether or not
they are interesting, relevant,
or delight us or other people,
or scare us or other people,
or thrill us or other people.
In other words, a testing of whether or not
there’s some fundamental rule to emerge.
Again, I’m going to repeat this many, many times
throughout this episode,
and I’m not going to apologize for that
because I think it’s so important to understand
that creativity is not just novel combinations.
They are novel combinations of things
that reveal something fundamental
and that often pop out to us.
If not every time,
certainly most of the time that we see that thing.
It almost never seems to be the case
that something truly creative dulls in its expression.
And that’s because what it’s repeating to us
over and over again is this fundamental rule
that normally we can’t see or hear or experience
in the absence of this creative act.
So the second part of creativity where things are tested
and where truly creative elements are discovered
is in convergent thinking.
And convergent thinking is, as the name suggests,
just the opposite of divergent thinking.
Convergent thinking would be, for example,
if I give you an image or I tell you the following things,
I say wing, water, and engine.
The concept that I happen to have in mind
is that of a plane that can land on water, right?
Most planes don’t land on water
or not intended to land on water.
One would hope that their plane doesn’t land on water
unless it’s a plane designed to land on water.
But in this case, a plane that can land on water
is one of the very few answers
that can combine wing, water, and engine, right?
I’m sure there are other answers.
There are other convergent thinking modes
that can take you to an answer that would be valid,
but there are not many.
And here, what’s really most important
is that I’m not asking you to spool out
or to radiate out from these three things.
Rather, I’m asking you to combine them in some way
that makes sense in the real world.
And indeed, there are planes that can land on water.
And wing, water, and engine combined within those things,
they are fundamental features.
They are in fact necessary, but not sufficient
for having a plane that can land on water.
Okay, so that’s just one example of convergent thinking.
And a convergent thinking task
would involve you being given a list of two or three
or maybe even five different things.
And then for each of those two or three
or five different things,
as quickly as you can to come up with a single answer
that binds all of those in a real world concept
that obey the laws of nature or physics in some way.
For instance, you could just come up with some, you know,
answer that said a bird that swallowed an engine
and that happens to be a seabird.
You could come up with that,
but that actually is not something that happens
or is that very typical at all.
And so it seems like kind of a mishmash
of things that are really just designed
for you to try and accomplish an answer
rather than something real,
such as a plane that lands on water, okay?
The point here is that divergent thinking
is one aspect of our cognition, of our thinking,
and convergent thinking is a very distinct aspect
of our cognition.
In fact, one of the critical requirements
for convergent thinking is also to access our memory banks
and our understanding about the outside world,
just as it were with divergent thinking,
but it requires more focus and more persistence.
In fact, if we were to come up with a key rule
for divergent thinking, it would be,
you almost want to have just enough focus
to remember what the initial object
or thing that was mentioned was to keep that in mind
so that your answers don’t become completely random.
But the more distant and everywhere in between
that you can generate answers,
that is the things that are very close to pens,
you know, black pen, red pen versus, you know,
pen and doorstop, pen acting as a doorstop.
Those are, one is very close.
Red pen is very close to black pen.
Doorstop is pretty far from black pen.
So that’s the idea is that you want to explore
and undergo a range of exploration of different ideas.
Whereas with convergent thinking,
you’re really trying to bind these things together.
And so the key element for convergent thinking
is the aspect of persistence and focus.
And that’s why convergent thinking in many ways
feels harder than divergent thinking.
It feels like there’s an answer
and I want to get the answer right and I can’t solve it.
It’s a puzzle and it’s a puzzle
that relies on very distinct brain circuits
from divergent thinking.
Which brain circuits?
Well, that’s what we’re going to describe next.
And again, this is not just going to be a list
of different brain circuits
with different names, doing different things.
That wouldn’t be useful to you or to me.
Rather, what you’re about to learn is truly incredible.
What it is, is we’re going to talk about
one single molecule, dopamine,
which is a molecule most typically associated
with motivation and desire and drive
and feelings of pleasure in some cases,
but that actually resides within
four different networks in the brain.
Today, we’re going to talk about two of those networks.
And dopamine acting in one network
directly underlies divergent thinking.
Whereas dopamine in another brain network
underlies convergent thinking.
And if at this point in this episode, you’re thinking,
okay, when am I going to get the tools
to understand creativity and how to be creative?
What I can assure you is that
if you understand divergent thinking,
which hopefully now you do,
and you can understand what convergent thinking is,
and you can understand that dopamine is responsible
for both divergent thinking and convergent thinking,
but through separate pathways,
well then, if you can understand
how those two separate pathways work
and how to engage them differentially,
therein lie the tools that you can use
both to explore ideas, in other words,
find what it is that could be creative,
and then systematically test each of those ideas
for what is truly creative.
That is what meets the criteria
for something that is novel and truly useful
and informs us about something
that we’ve never seen, heard, or felt before.
Let’s just take a moment to talk about
the incredible molecule that is dopamine.
Many people are familiar with dopamine
from the concept of quote-unquote dopamine hits,
which is popular language describing
the feeling of pleasure that we get
from pretty much anything that we like
or that we continue to engage in repeatedly.
So some people will talk about the dopamine hit
that they get from somebody attractive
that they like texting them back,
or the dopamine hit that they get from social media,
or the dopamine hit that they get from sugar,
or the dopamine hit that they get from this or from that.
To be honest, the concept of dopamine hits
is not one that I favor because in general,
whenever people talk about dopamine hits,
typically they’re talking about activities
such as social media,
which dopamine may be involved at some level,
but often it’s the case
that the behavior associated with that thing,
in this case, social media,
is more of the compulsive nature
rather than an active seeking of something
with positive anticipation.
And that’s really what dopamine is about,
at least in the context of one of its major functions
in the brain.
Dopamine is really about motivation and desire and movement.
And it makes sense why motivation, desire, and movement
would be linked up through a common,
in this case, neuromodulator or chemical like dopamine,
because throughout evolution,
if we were excited for or motivated to pursue something,
we had to move in order to get it, to obtain it.
And in general, we can frame dopamine under the umbrella
of dopamine tends to be involved
in neural circuits in the brain
that are involved in processes
that are taking us beyond the confines of our skin.
That is, that motivate us to go do something
in terms of action in the world.
Now, that statement might seem distantly placed
from a discussion about creativity,
but as we’ll learn a little bit later,
one of the most useful tools for engaging creativity
and becoming more creative
is to think about action elements within a narrative.
That is things that we and others can do
in order to discover new rules through actual movement.
That’s a little bit cryptic, forgive me,
but I promise I’ll return to it later
and I will make it crystal clear.
There are four major circuits in the brain that use dopamine
although I should mention
there are additional circuits as well.
In fact, your eye even contains neurons
that release dopamine that control the sensitivity
of your eye at different times of day to light, et cetera.
The four major circuits in the brain
that utilize dopamine, however,
are used for four major purposes.
And I’ll describe what those are.
First of all, is a neural circuit that uses dopamine
among other things, but certainly relies on dopamine
in a critical way to engage movement,
including eye movements.
And we will return to eye movements
and why they’re so important for understanding creativity
and maybe even for generating creativity a little bit later.
The name of the circuit, again,
is less important than what it does,
but the name of this circuit, for those that want to know,
is the so-called nigrostriatal pathway, okay?
The substantia nigra is a brain area that is very dark
that projects to an area called the dorsal striatum.
It contains a bunch of sub-regions.
So again, for those of you
that really geek out on this stuff, great.
You can learn these names and retain them in your memory.
If you don’t care about names, don’t worry about it.
Just discard the names.
But areas of the brain like the caudate and putatum
and the dorsal striatum receive input
from the substantia nigra.
In neuroanatomy, when we name something,
we say the origin of that thing and where it connects through.
So nigrostriatal tells you that there is a connection
between the substantia nigra,
because it came first, nigrostriatal,
and then striatal is where it ends up.
So nigrostriatal pathway is involved
in generating bodily movements.
It’s involved in eye movements,
and it is actually a brain area that’s engaged
when you think about movement.
Even just have a story in your mind about walking
or a story in your mind about running
or a story in your mind about driving,
this area is engaged.
Very interesting brain area.
So that’s the first circuit.
Very important to understand.
And I’ll tell you right now,
that is the brain circuit that is engaged
when you undergo divergent thinking.
Now that itself should be interesting, right?
Even if you don’t remember any of the names
of the things I just told you,
that you have a brain circuit
that even if you just think about walking,
it becomes more active,
and the dopamine is involved in that brain activity.
And if you recall,
divergent thinking involves taking a concept
as boring as a pen and thinking about other concepts
that could link up with that pen in some sort of way,
logical or illogical, right?
The bridge could be completely abstract
and really fantastical with a bunch of different ideas
in between, like a pen acting as a doorstop
because of some situation
where you need to run downstairs in a fire
and get back upstairs quickly
to rescue somebody very divergent
or as divergent as black pen to red pen.
But what’s amazing is that that same circuit
is the one that’s involved in physical movement,
in generating and thinking about physical movement.
That turns out to be vitally important
for tapping into the creativity process.
So really frame that up in your mind
or commit it to memory.
Now, the second dopamine circuit associated with creativity
is the one associated with convergent thinking,
which again is the kind of thinking
where there’s a specific correct answer,
it requires focus and it requires persistence.
And the name of that circuit,
again, the name isn’t as important as what it does,
but the name of that circuit is the mesocortical pathway.
The mesocortical pathway is involved in motivation
and it has an emotional component too.
Now, it will become clear in a few minutes
why that emotional component is vital.
But this is a circuit that originates in a brain structure
called the lateral ventral tegmental area.
Again, a bunch of words,
you can remember it if you want,
lateral ventral tegmental area,
or you can not worry about the name.
And it connects to the prefrontal cortex,
that area just behind the forehead.
And this mesocortical area
is involved in motivation and emotion
and is critical for focus and persistence.
It is distinct from a very nearby area,
just sitting right next door,
the so-called mesolimbic area,
which is involved in desire and feelings of reward.
And this is the area that is associated more typically
with addictive behaviors or compulsive behaviors.
We’re going to leave out the discussion
about the mesolimbic pathway for now,
because it’s not critical to divergent
or convergent thinking.
And it’s not critical to the process of creativity,
at least as far as we know.
But I mentioned it because it is the third
in the four dopaminergic circuits.
And then the fourth circuit,
certainly one I’ve never talked about before
in this podcast, which doesn’t mean anything
except that we haven’t gotten to it yet,
is the tubero-infrandibular pathway.
And that is the pathway associated with dopamine
and your pituitary gland and the release of hormones
in particular that travel to the ovary.
If you have ovaries or to your testes,
if you have testes and trigger the release of things
like estrogen and testosterone, et cetera,
dopamine is intimately involved in that circuitry.
Again, not the topic of today’s discussion.
For today’s discussion,
we want to remember that there’s a dopamine circuit
called the nigrostriatal circuit,
which is involved in movement and divergent thinking.
And that alone should set a flag up for you.
I’m like, wow, just thinking about new ideas
has something to do with movement, with physical movement.
And the dopamine circuit that is the mesocortical pathway,
which is the one that’s associated
with motivation and emotion.
And that’s the one required for persistence and focus
for convergent thinking.
Why am I telling you all of this about dopamine?
Well, it turns out that dopamine
creates a certain number of responses in the brain and body
when it is active in one or the other of these circuits.
And just for sake of simplicity,
so I don’t have to keep saying nigrostriatal
and mesocortical, here going forward,
I’m going to talk about the dopamine circuit
that’s associated with divergent thinking
or the dopamine circuit associated with convergent thinking.
And again, divergent thinking and convergent thinking
are the two processes that have to occur,
usually first divergent, then convergent thinking,
then back and forth and back and forth
in order to arrive at something creative.
Divergent thinking is about exploration.
Convergent thinking is about testing things
and coming up with things that are the right answer,
that feel right.
And we will better define what right means
a little bit later, but you already sort of know.
Right in this context is when you have
some combination of elements or some idea
or some written passage or some music
or some physical action that you just know,
this is really novel and really cool,
or people see it or hear it or taste it
and say, this is really novel and really cool.
And they don’t necessarily know why,
it’s just different in a way that feels true.
I’d like to take a brief break
and thank our sponsor, InsideTracker.
InsideTracker is a personalized nutrition platform
that analyzes data from your blood and DNA
to help you better understand your body
and help you reach your health goals.
I’ve long been a believer in getting regular blood work done
for the simple reason that many of the factors
that impact your immediate and long-term health
can only be analyzed from a quality blood test.
The problem with a lot of blood and DNA tests out there,
however, is that you get data back about metabolic factors,
lipids and hormones and so forth,
but you don’t know what to do with those data.
InsideTracker solves that problem
and makes it very easy for you to understand
what sorts of nutritional, behavioral,
maybe even supplementation-based interventions
you might want to take on
in order to adjust the numbers of those metabolic factors,
hormones, lipids, and other things
that impact your immediate and long-term health
to bring those numbers into the ranges that are appropriate
and indeed optimal for you.
If you’d like to try InsideTracker,
you can visit insidetracker.com slash Huberman
and get 20% off any of InsideTracker’s plans.
That’s insidetracker.com slash Huberman to get 20% off.
Now, I realize that for some of you
listening to this episode,
we are probably at the point along the pathway
of concept and definition and mechanism
that leaves you in a place of real wanting a tool.
And so I promise that I’m going to get into more tools,
but to satisfy you and to make sure
that you do indeed understand that there are tools
that can emerge from the information
that you already now have in mind,
I do want to share with you one particular tool
from the literature that has been demonstrated
over and over again to support and build
and enhance divergent thinking.
And I also want to share with you a tool
that has been shown from the scientific literature
to enhance convergent thinking,
because both convergent and divergent thinking
are critical for the creative process.
Now, I should emphasize that some people out there,
either by training or by genetics or by both,
will be naturally better
at divergent or convergent thinking.
And in fact, we now know in a kind of almost poetic
kind of way that naturally occurring variations in genes,
which underlie naturally occurring variations
in the percentage of dopamine
in one set of brain circuits versus another,
do seem to relate to whether or not people
are naturally good at divergent thinking
or convergent thinking.
Now, that’s a very nature-based explanation
for why some people are better at divergent thinking
and other people are better at convergent thinking.
Nature and nurture is something
that can never really be teased apart exactly,
because of course, if someone has a natural proclivity
for something based on their genes,
you can’t often separate that from their parents
because we inherit our genes from our parents.
Although, even in cases where people are raised away
from their parents through adoption, et cetera,
it’s very hard to separate nature and nurture
because somebody with a natural proclivity for things
might engage in those things more, et cetera, et cetera.
The point is that for those of you
that are very, very good at divergent thinking
or very, very good at convergent thinking,
some of that might’ve been inherited,
but more than likely some of that depended
on the kinds of activities that you engaged in
in your early years,
in particular in the years between age five and 25.
And for those of you that are aged between five and 25,
all I can say is please learn to engage
both divergent and convergent thinking
as much as possible
because you will enhance your ability for both.
For those of you 25 and older,
you can still enhance your ability
to engage divergent and convergent thinking.
And the fortunate news, the equalizer, I should say,
is that regardless of whether or not
you are naturally better at divergent or convergent thinking
or you acquired it through activities,
you need both in order to be creative.
So what we know is that in order to engage
divergent thinking, we need access to our memory banks.
We need to come up with possibilities
and those possibilities can only come
from what’s contained within our memory systems
of our brain, areas like the hippocampus, et cetera.
But the names again, don’t matter.
We just know that if we are going to come up
with novel combinations of things or novel uses of things
or totally new ideas about how objects or notes of music
or foods or tastes or whatever can be combined,
we have to do that with preexisting knowledge.
And yet what we need to do
in order to engage divergent thinking
is suppress what is called autobiographical narratives.
And in particular autobiographical narratives,
we need to discard with judgments
about how certain combinations of things
impacted us in the past.
Now, this is, I think is what people mean
when they encourage the exploration of creativity
by so-called boundary exploration.
You hear about this a lot
in kind of the self-help and psychology literature.
And I’m not at all disparaging of that literature,
although rarely does it define exactly how and why
to go about being more creative in the,
or in this case, to be more divergent in our thinking.
So they’ll say, you have to take risks
or you have to suppress judgment,
but how do you actually do that?
Well, there’s a wonderful paper
that talks about one way to do it.
One way to do it is what’s called
open monitoring meditation
or even just open monitoring thinking.
And just to make what could otherwise
be a somewhat complex section here very simple,
what I’ll also tell you
is that if you want to enhance convergent thinking,
you can do that a number of ways,
but you can do that in particular
by doing a different type of meditation or thought process,
which is called focused attention meditation.
So let’s talk about open monitoring meditation
and why it’s so useful for enhancing divergent thinking,
this critical element of the creative process.
First of all, open monitoring meditation
and focused attention meditation
can be performed the exact same way physically.
You can sit there, eyes closed.
I don’t care if you’re in a lotus position,
it doesn’t really matter.
You’re lying down, you’re standing up.
You could in theory do open monitoring meditation
with eyes open,
and that would be an interesting variant on it.
But for sake of the discussion right now,
let’s just focus on the study
that talks about these specific tools
and the way that they were used in the study.
The title of the paper that I’m essentially summarizing
is called open monitoring meditation
reduces the involvement of brain regions
related to memory function.
Now, right off the bat,
that should cue you to something interesting.
Something about divergent thinking and open monitoring
is related to suppressing memory.
But as you recall, just a few moments ago,
I said that in order to engage in divergent thinking,
you need to kind of kill off the narratives
of what has to be related to what
and come up with new narratives.
You still need to understand possibilities,
but you need to forget prior understanding
of what those possibilities have to be
and start thinking about what those possibilities could be.
And so that it turns out involves suppression
of certain brain areas.
Open monitoring meditation is typically done
for about 10 to 30 minutes, although it could be longer.
And unlike other forms of meditation
where you sit and concentrate on your breathing
and trying to redirect your thinking back to your breathing
or to your posture or to a chant or a mantra,
open monitoring meditation is simply a matter
of having you sit there or lie down, close your eyes
and to allow whatever surfaces in your mind to surface.
And what you practice is the practice of non-judgment.
Now, non-judgment itself is a little bit of an abstract theme
because of course, the moment you say, don’t judge,
you and others start to judge.
It’s just the way that the brain works.
You say, don’t think about an elephant,
you think about an elephant, that’s a perfectly natural.
You go to an edge of a bridge or a cliff
and you think about jumping off
even though you don’t, please don’t, jump off.
And that’s because it’s part of the circuitry
that’s keeping you from jumping off
is the thought about what would happen if you did, okay?
So open monitoring meditation involves dedicating
a certain amount of time where you close your eyes
and whatever thoughts arise, whatever emotions arise,
whatever ideas arise, to watch those
and take an inventory of them,
to just merely watch them show up and pass
or maybe you become fixated on them for some period of time
or maybe even just one for a long period of time.
All of that is fine.
In other words, whatever surfaces, surfaces.
That’s open monitoring meditation.
And that we know from brain imaging studies
and we know from measurements of dopamine
in particular brain circuits.
And we know from people who train
with open monitoring meditation on a regular basis
improves divergent thinking capability.
So in terms of tools, practicing open monitoring meditation
or what I would just call open monitoring thinking
is going to be immensely useful.
And this is actually an opportunity to cue up something
that I mentioned in our episode on meditation
which goes deep into the different kinds of meditation
involving focus inward and outward, et cetera.
You’re welcome to check out that episode.
It’s at hubermanlab.com.
But the point is that rather than think about
the word meditation, which carries a bunch of ideas
about what it is and what it isn’t and how to do it,
meditation is really just a perceptual exercise.
For instance, you could do a meditation
where you look at a single point on a wall for five minutes
and redirect your focus to that single point on a wall
over and over again every time your mind drifts
as it no doubt would, or to a tone in the room,
you can attend to that and redirect to that.
Rather than think about as a meditation,
it’s really just a perceptual exercise.
That’s all that meditation is.
So open monitoring meditation
is really just a form of perception
where you’re paying attention,
you’re perceiving your thoughts
without laying judgment to those thoughts
or trying not to lay judgment to those thoughts.
And what people find is that they very quickly
within a few days get better
at doing open monitoring meditation.
And fortunately within just a few days
and certainly within about a week or more of practice,
and it doesn’t even have to be daily practice.
So although of course daily practice
will accelerate the process further,
people become significantly better at divergent thinking.
And that’s because of the dopamine circuits
and in particular along the nigrostriatal pathway
becoming more active.
And the wonderful thing is that when you repeat a practice
and a particular neural circuit
is engaged over and over again deliberately,
that neural circuit becomes easier to engage,
so-called neuroplasticity.
So I would encourage any of you
that want to explore the creative process
for whatever reason or get better at the creative process,
dedicate some amount of time,
maybe even just five minutes every other day
to doing this open monitoring meditation.
I’ve tried this meditation,
it’s actually quite fun to do
because at least to me it feels a lot easier
than the meditation associated with convergent thinking.
Now, the convergent thinking meditation
is the so-called focus attention meditation.
And that’s also described in the same study
and other studies have explored
which particular brain networks it involves.
And I can just tell you that focused attention meditation,
which you can think of,
or I’d prefer that you think of
just as a perceptual exercise,
involves sitting or lying down,
closing your eyes, focusing either on your breath
or some element of your body,
could be the tops of your knees or the clasp of your hands.
It could be focusing on an auditory tone,
you could even do it eyes open
and stare at a point on a wall or a flame of light,
whatever it happens to be
that allows you to redirect your focus
to a particular location or idea or sound,
that is known to improve your ability
to engage convergent thinking,
to quickly parse through
or analyze a bunch of different choices
and to persist in choice selection
and therefore more rapidly arrive at the correct answer.
This is well-established and in fact,
in the episode that I did with a wonderful guest,
Dr. Wendy Suzuki from New York University,
she talked about how a daily meditation
of about 10 to 13 minutes performed for about eight weeks,
that’s what they explored.
And that study greatly increases people’s ability
to focus and in fact, their memory.
And that’s exactly the point,
which is that convergent thinking,
as I mentioned before,
requires persistence, focus and access to specific memories.
So if you are somebody who wants to get better at focusing,
that is the meditation for you.
However, because today we’re talking about creativity,
if you are somebody who wants to get better
at divergent thinking and convergent thinking,
the two elements of creativity, that is,
I would encourage you to do a dual meditation,
that is a meditation that starts with open monitoring
for maybe five to 10 minutes
and then transitions to focused attention
for maybe five to 10 minutes.
Because the positioning of divergent thinking
and then convergent thinking close together,
more closely resembles what the creative process really is
and what it typically involves.
Most of us would love to have a situation
where we can spend a morning or a day or a week
brainstorming, just kind of brainstorming.
Whatever we think about is fine, that’s divergent thinking.
Whatever elements, just throw them up on the whiteboard.
We sometimes see people and companies doing this at retreats
and you bring people into a novel environment.
You say, let’s just forget all the rules
and let’s just come up with new ideas about something,
new uses of something, new strategies,
and nothing is too crazy, nothing’s off limits.
And sure, that’s a useful exercise, so-called brainstorming.
But at some point,
there’s the requirement to cross off things.
And typically that’s done later in the retreat
or later in the meeting or later in the weekend.
And that’s a wonderful way to approach creativity
and to try and be creative.
But not a lot of people train for that on a regular basis.
So what I just described to you
are research tested tools for training
for divergent thinking and convergent thinking.
And I would encourage people who are interested
in being more creative to try and do these
on a somewhat regular basis.
If not every day,
then certainly a few times a week or more.
Certainly the more you do it,
the better you’re going to get at it.
That’s well demonstrated in the literature.
And if you’re somebody who’s very consistent
doing maybe five minutes of open monitoring meditation
and five minutes immediately after
of focused attention meditation daily,
you can expect that you will get very, very good
at these processes very, very quickly.
Now, I’m not going to go into a lengthy description
of the different lines of evidence
that the corresponding areas of the brain are active
in each of these different kinds of meditation.
But what I can tell you is that there’ve been
some beautiful, what are called loss of function studies
where particular brain areas are either depleted of dopamine
or where dopamine in some cases,
I guess what we would call gain of function studies,
although not the kind of gain of function studies
associated with virology,
different gain of function studies
where you enhance the level of dopamine in the brain.
What you find is that both divergent
and convergent thinking are enhanced
when levels of dopamine are elevated.
Now, we’re not necessarily talking about pharmacology here.
It turns out that there are other ways to elevate dopamine
that make us better at divergent and convergent thinking
in particular by using mood.
And now I’d like to talk about how,
what mood you are in
when you happen to start a creative process
or try and do a sort of training
such as open monitoring meditation or focus meditation,
how your mood relates to your level of dopamine at baseline,
what we call your sort of tonic as it’s called,
meaning consistent or ongoing level of dopamine,
how that dictates whether or not you are going to be better
at one particular aspect of the creative process or another
and how you can enhance your creativity
in the very short term, very quickly using tools
that are known to trigger additional release of dopamine,
which in some cases is good
and in some cases is bad, I should mention.
And in other words,
determine how you feel in one moment
should dictate what sort of tool you should use
in order to become more creative.
The relationship between mood and creativity
is a fascinating one that is bridged by one main feature,
which is the amount of dopamine present
in this nigrostriatal pathway.
And there’s a really wonderful correlate
or measure of the amount of dopamine
that’s active in that pathway
that can be addressed non-invasively in the laboratory.
As I mentioned, the nigrostriatal pathway
is involved in movement and in eye blinking,
which of course is a movement.
It’s not a movement of the sort that we typically think of
when we think of movements,
but nonetheless it relies on dopamine levels in this pathway
and in fact, we can state very confidently
that when dopamine levels are elevated,
the blinking reflex is more active.
People just blink more.
When dopamine levels are lower
or less active in this pathway,
people tend to blink less.
So blink frequency is a common measure
in studies of dopamine within this pathway
that relate to creativity.
The work that I’m about to describe
is largely the work of two authors
who have done wonderful work across several papers.
Unfortunately for me, their names are difficult to pronounce
so I apologize to them and their relatives
for what is sure to be incorrect pronunciation.
But the last names of these authors
are Cermahini and Hommel.
They’re in the Netherlands.
So Cermahini and Hommel done a number of different papers
or studies rather of the relationship
between blinking mood and creativity,
in particular divergent thinking.
What they found is that if people are blinking fairly often
and they measure their mood through subjective tests
and if they were to do brain imaging,
which other studies have done,
they find is that those people
can engage in divergent thinking very easily.
In other words, being in a good mood
facilitates divergent thinking.
Now, some of you might immediately say, well, duh.
If you’re in a good mood,
you can kind of be more playful about the exploration
about what could happen with these notes of music
or these foods, et cetera.
But it’s not so obvious because it turns out
that if your dopamine levels are very, very high
and this can be measured non-invasively
through the frequency of blinks
or it can be measured more invasively through brain imaging,
even through blood draws
or other methods to measure dopamine.
If dopamine levels are very, very high,
what you observe is that divergent thinking
is actually very, very poor.
Now, a naturally occurring, truly pathological example
of this would be something like manic bipolar disorder
where somebody is in the manic phase
or somebody who has taken methamphetamine or cocaine.
What tends to happen is that they have lots
and lots of ideas.
All of those ideas seem really exciting to them.
But if you were to talk to them for any given moment,
they would be very fixated
on one particular tunnel of ideas.
And by being fixated on one particular tunnel of ideas,
like the idea that they’re going to run
for president tomorrow.
This is unfortunately typical of people who have bipolar,
which is not to say that everybody who runs
for president is bipolar.
Rather, people who are bipolar often have these delusions
of grandeur that they’re somehow going to be president
simply because they decided to
and that they were selected to do this, et cetera, et cetera.
Ideas about themselves and other people
that are very constrained.
In other words, not very divergent.
So divergent thinking is favored
by having elevated levels of dopamine, but not too high.
Well, that of course creates a conundrum.
How do you know how much dopamine you need
and how to achieve those elevated levels of dopamine?
Well, leaving aside people who are suffering
from a manic episode,
what Cermahini and Homel have discovered is
that if people are in sort of a low mood,
they’re not feeling great.
Maybe they’re depressed,
but they’re just not feeling that great.
They feel, you know, on a scale of one to 10
around a two or a three, maybe a four.
The probability that they will be able to engage effectively
in divergent thinking is quite low.
However, the good news is they are typically
very susceptible to elevations in mood
through observing or hearing positive stories,
listening to music that they like,
any kind of so-called inspirational stimuli.
What this means is that
if you’re somebody who’s not feeling very motivated
to engage in divergent thinking,
you’re not feeling very creative,
you’re feeling a little low,
the thing to do in that case is actually
to take external stimuli,
things that you know that you like
and start interacting with those stimuli
to get your mood elevated
and then to engage in divergent thinking.
However, what Cermahini and Homel have also shown is
that if people are already in a very good mood,
elevating dopamine further is not conducive
and in fact is detrimental to divergent thinking.
And in that case, they would be better off,
for example, not engaging in any activities
or you know, taking anything in the way of pharmacology
that would further increase their dopamine
and probably limiting the amount of external stimuli
that are coming in through music and visual stimuli
and really focusing on divergent thinking
in the creative process immediately.
Now, this is important.
In an earlier episode,
both on bipolar and on other forms of depression,
I talked about how rates of bipolar manic episodes
and dopamine levels and creativity tend to be correlated.
Now, unfortunately, rates of suicide
are 20 to 30 times higher
in people who have bipolar disorder as well.
And so there’s a whole dark side to the bipolar disorder
that makes it a very, very dangerous
and important disorder to treat.
But for sake of the discussion of creativity,
what this means is that we all need to develop
some sort of intuitive sense
as to whether or not our mood is,
suppose we could bend this into three categories,
is kind of yes, you know, happy, excited, positive mood.
And of course, there are going to be levels to that.
Low, kind of like mm, or kind of meh,
kind of in the middle.
So if you’re in a low mood or kind of meh mood,
by all means, engage in something
probably for about five to 30 minutes
that elevates your mood
before trying to engage in divergent thinking.
However, if you happen to be in a pretty positive mood,
even if you’re not 10 out of 10 on mood,
then bringing in additional stimuli
to increase your levels of dopamine will not help you
and in fact can hurt the divergent thinking process.
So in that case, I would also encourage you
to think about something that was discussed
on a previous episode,
which is the particular effects of caffeine.
I’ll get into caffeine a little bit later,
but just very briefly,
caffeine increases levels of dopamine receptors.
So it’s not that caffeine is bad.
In fact, caffeine can be neuroprotective,
it can enhance focus and so forth.
But divergent thinking is sort of anti-focus.
It requires just enough focus
to be able to come up with new ideas,
but you actually don’t want to be overly focused.
Focus is more conducive to conversion thinking.
In fact, that’s exactly what the literature shows,
is that caffeine,
because its effects on epinephrine and related systems
in the brain like adenosine,
but mainly because of its effects on persistence and focus
is very conducive to convergent thinking.
So if you’re somebody who wants to explore creativity
and wants to get better at creativity,
you now know that you need to engage in divergent thinking
and then afterwards convergent thinking.
I would recommend not using stimulants such as caffeine
prior to divergent thinking,
but rather use stimulants
if you do want to use stimulants such as caffeine
prior to convergent thinking.
And in fact, in formulating the architecture
of today’s episode,
which took me many hours across many different days,
I confess, I actually decided to try this.
In trying to imagine the different configurations
and ways that this information can be organized,
I deliberately abstained from caffeine
during those bouts of work.
And when structuring everything
according to the decisions I had already made,
I purposely ingested caffeine prior to that.
Now, of course, constructing a podcast episode
is not really the ultimate example of a creative act
because of course it’s taking existing information,
it’s arranging it in novel ways,
but it doesn’t necessarily allow key concepts
to pop out in the way that for instance,
Banksy or Rothko or an Escher would pop out, okay?
I’m certainly not naive in thinking that it does,
but the principle is what’s important here.
You need divergent thinking, you need convergent thinking.
You need some level of elevated dopamine
in order to engage in divergent thinking,
but not so high that it starts to inhibit that process.
Now, if you were to come into the laboratory,
this could be measured by your frequency of blinking.
For better, for worse,
we can’t actually count the number of times that we blink
unless we’re actively paying attention to it.
So I don’t recommend that you pay attention to your blinking
because that will take you off course
from all the other important things of your life.
And how many times you’re blinking
is rarely an important thing for you to pay attention to.
You can, however, learn to calibrate your mood,
that is to assess your mood,
whether or not you’re in low, medium, or high mood,
no problem using that broad binning, right?
You could scale it on one to 10,
and then decide whether or not you’re going to use
some dopamine elevating stimulus from the outside.
Again, it could be music,
could be exercise is an excellent way to elevate dopamine.
I’ll talk about another well-established one
from the research literature
that is known to elevate dopamine by 65%
in the particular pathway
that’s relevant for divergent thinking,
and to do that without any pharmacology.
I’ll share that with you in a moment,
but you need to decide for you in a given moment
or in a given work attempt at creativity,
what you need and apply accordingly,
because as Ciaramini and Hummel have shown,
whether or not you are in a low mood, medium mood,
or high mood really can determine whether or not
you’ll be able to access divergent thinking or not.
Now, if you’re somebody who already has an idea in mind,
you’re very excited about a creative idea
and you want to hone it, you want to shape it,
you want to pressure test it.
We’ll talk a little bit more about what that means
in a three-step process in just a little bit.
I would strongly encourage you to look at that process
as a very linear process
in which there are right and wrong answers.
And there, the use of caffeine at appropriate dosages
and dosages for caffeine that are safe,
and in fact, performance enhancing,
we’re covered in the episode on caffeine.
Turns out it’s one to three milligrams per kilogram
of body weight, by the way.
And if you want to leverage caffeine
or maybe even other forms of healthy legal stimulants,
those are covered in the caffeine episode
and I’ll talk about a few more a little bit later.
So to summarize this segment
and also just to make a more general point,
I think it’s very useful for people to start
to pay attention to what their tonic level,
that is their baseline level of dopamine,
ought to be in this nigrostriatal circuit
and in other circuits.
And to do that by learning to assess one’s mood
and pay attention to what kind of mood
they happen to be in and then to leverage tools,
behavioral tools, maybe pharmacologic tools,
provided they’re safe and they’re legal,
in order to either increase dopamine
or to elect not to increase dopamine
in order to access the creative process.
Now, I’ve mentioned pharmacology a few times.
I’d like to talk about that just a little bit more
in the context of dopamine.
First of all, there is no supplement or drug
that you or anyone else can take
that will selectively elevate dopamine
in only one of the four circuits that I described before.
Okay, this is just the state of the technology nowadays.
If you take a pill
or even if you were to inject some substance,
again, I hope this would be legal and safe, et cetera,
whatever mode of delivery,
there is no technology that exists at this time
that would allow you to selectively amplify dopamine,
for instance, just in the nigrostriatal pathway
or just in the mesocortical pathway.
Again, the nigrostriatal pathway
associated with divergent thinking,
the mesocortical pathway associated
with cognitive persistence and convergent thinking.
If you were to amplify dopamine levels,
for instance, by taking the amino acid precursor
to dopamine L-tyrosine,
something that I occasionally do
to enhance dopamine levels for sake of work or energy,
500 milligrams or 1,000 milligrams even of L-tyrosine,
sometimes I’ll combine that with other things like alpha-GPC,
it’s going to enhance dopamine transmission
in the nigrostriatal pathway,
the mesocortical pathway,
but also in the mesolimbic pathway,
and also for that matter,
in the tuberoinframedibular pathway
associated with the pituitary.
There is no way to direct dopamine activation
to just one of those pathways.
That’s just a reflection of the existing technology.
Now, this is also true if you rely on illicit drugs
to increase dopamine.
So if it’s cocaine or methamphetamine,
those will greatly increase dopamine,
but non-selectively across all those different pathways.
And likewise, with any drugs that inhibit or block
or antagonizes, it’s called dopamine.
This is why people who, for instance,
have schizophrenia and take drugs
to suppress auditory hallucinations,
some of those drugs work
because they block the so-called D2 receptor
of the dopamine pathway.
D2 receptors are present in all four
of the dopaminergic pathways in the brain.
And oftentimes, those drugs will in fact
suppress psychotic symptoms, auditory hallucinations,
et cetera, because they reduce dopamine,
but those people oftentimes
will have problems with movement.
They will express what’s called
in the clinical literature tardive dyskinesia,
kind of writhing of the face and the body
from suppression of dopamine
within the nigrostriatal pathway,
which is associated with movement.
They will sometimes have deficits in eye blinking.
People with Parkinson’s who actually have selective deficits
of dopamine within the substantia nigra,
nigrostriatal, remember, substantia nigra,
show deficits in what?
In movement, in the smoothness of movement.
Oftentimes they won’t blink at all.
They’ll have kind of a blank stare
and they have other issues as well.
So if you’re somebody who’s interested
in increasing dopamine through the use
of legal safe pharmacology,
as I would hope it would be the case,
there are ways to do that reasonably safely
for most people.
Again, people with bipolar disorder
or issues with the dopaminergic pathway should not do this.
I know nowadays there’s a lot of use
of drugs that increase dopamine,
such as Ritalin, Adderall, Modafinil, R-Modafinil,
often prescribed for things like
attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.
We did an entire episode on ADHD
and pharmacologic prescription supplement
and behavioral and nutritional tools for ADHD.
You can find that episode at hubermanlab.com.
I know a number of people take those compounds
in order to increase dopamine and focus
for sake of studying or other activities,
staying up long hours, et cetera.
And the fact that they increase focus,
although they do have their side effects,
sometimes severe, sometimes habit forming,
sometimes even addicting as well.
But the fact that they increase focus
should automatically tell you something,
that those drugs in particular increase dopamine
in the so-called mesocortical and mesolimbic pathways.
Why can I say that?
How can I say that with any degree of confidence?
Well, there are these four pathways,
one’s involved in movement,
but these other ones are involved in motivation
and desire and reward.
And I told you that these things can be habit forming
and addicting in some cases,
and they can greatly increase focus.
And focus is supported by enhanced levels of dopamine
within this mesolimbic and mesocortical pathway.
So yes, those drugs increase dopamine across the board,
but there does seem to be some weighting of dopamine
toward the systems involved in motivation and reward,
and sometimes even leading to habit formation and addiction.
That’s why those drugs should only be taken
with the close supervision of a very skilled psychiatrist
or somebody else who’s board certified
who can really govern that.
There are, however, ways to increase dopamine
more evenly across the board
using non-prescription approaches.
And one I already mentioned, which is L-tyrosine,
taken typically in dosages of 500 to 1,000 milligrams.
L-tyrosine is not as potent in increasing dopamine
as are the prescription drugs that I referred to before,
tends to be milder for some people,
it can have a very amplified effect,
they feel it right away,
it’s very intense in elevating focus and motivation
and the desire to move.
For other people, it’s less potent.
It really depends on a number of things.
I should mention that regular consumption of caffeine
of one to three milligrams per kilogram
of body weight per day
also will increase dopamine receptor efficacy and density,
which will make any existing dopamine more effective,
whether or not that dopamine is triggered
by things like L-tyrosine,
or if you’re not taking anything to elevate dopamine,
the dopamine that you do make will be more effective
in elevating your mood, motivation, and desire to move,
and by extension, divergent thinking,
if you are consuming caffeine.
But again, caffeine should be taken
prior to convergent thinking type tasks
probably more than it should be taken
prior to divergent thinking tasks.
And of course, there are other legal supplements
that can elevate dopamine as well.
In particular, phenylethylamine is very effective
in doing that, 600 milligrams of that,
has a brief effect lasting only about 30 to 45 minutes,
but it is one that many people find beneficial
for sake of studying or for creative thinking
and so on and so forth.
Now that’s pharmacology,
and in fact, there’s an extensive landscape of prescription
and supplement-based pharmacology and indeed nutrition.
For instance, the consumption of foods
that are high in L-tyrosine,
such as aged Parmesan cheese, for instance, of all things,
very, very high in L-tyrosine,
the precursor to dopamine.
Certain foods, you can look up online
which foods contain high levels of L-tyrosine
and which ones are compatible with your nutrition.
But leaving pharmacology aside,
there’s a very exciting non-pharmacological tool,
a purely behavioral tool,
that the research literature has told us
can selectively increase dopamine
within the nigrostriatal pathway,
the pathway that’s involved in divergent thinking,
and can do so very dramatically,
as much as 65% above baseline.
And so this is a behavioral tool
that is useful for a number of things,
but that I find particularly interesting
in leveraging towards the exploration
and enhancement of creativity,
because first of all, it’s purely behavioral,
so it’s zero cost,
and it involves no manipulation of brain neuromodulators
or chemistry through pharmacology.
So it’s something that you can explore very safely
and certainly not having to purchase anything.
And what’s really remarkable is the selectivity,
or I think it’s fair to say the immense selectivity
that this particular behavioral intervention
seems to exert on dopamine within this pathway
associated with divergent thinking.
So the study that I’m about to describe
is a study that dates back 20 years.
Now that should not concern you.
In fact, the early arrival of this study,
or what now seems to be early arrival,
I mean, it wasn’t that long ago,
is really exciting because the first line of this study
really illustrates how important
or how much of a landmark study this really is.
And so I’ll just read you the first line of the study,
then I’ll tell you the title,
then I’ll tell you what they discovered
in fairly top contour,
and we will provide a link to the study
if you want to peruse it in more detail.
The first line of the study is,
this is the first in vivo, just meaning in the organism.
In this case, this was a study on humans.
This is the first in vivo demonstration
of an association between
an endogenous neurotransmitter release,
endogenous means within us, and conscious experience.
So what this sentence essentially says
is this is the first study exploring
how a chemical that’s naturally released in our body
relates to a particular quality of conscious experience.
This study was performed in Scandinavia
in one of the hospitals in Denmark.
Again, we’ll provide a link.
The first author is Kjer,
I think I’m pronouncing it correctly,
although probably not, K-J-A-E-R, et al.
And the title of the study is
Increased Dopamine Tone
During Meditation-Induced Change of Consciousness.
And I want to just highlight
that the meditation used in this study
isn’t really a meditation at all.
I don’t know why they selected that for the title.
The behavioral protocol used in this study
was more akin to what is normally called yoga nidra,
or NSDR, non-sleep deep rest.
Now, yoga nidra and NSDR have been discussed
many times before on this podcast.
Yoga nidra, for instance, is a practice
that’s been around for hundreds, if not thousands of years,
in which people deliberately lie still.
So they’re forcing themselves to be mostly motionless.
Small movements are fine.
And they’re directing their attention
to the surface of their body.
They’re doing long exhale breathing,
sometimes some intentions, sometimes some visualization,
but it’s really self-directed relaxation.
And the key component is that people stay awake
and engage in very little movement.
And the keyword there is movement.
Now, non-sleep deep rest is an acronym,
a term that I coined.
It’s not a term that I coined
in order to try and wipe away or discard with yoga nidra.
I’m a person who has great respect
for yoga nidra and its traditions.
It’s a term that I coined
in order to encompass a number of practices
that don’t include any mystical-type language
or scientific language, for that matter,
and that doesn’t involve intentions.
It involves deep relaxation,
yet remaining wide awake and conscious.
Sometimes people fall asleep and that’s okay,
but this is really an atypical brain state
of being deeply relaxed, yet in general awake,
and motionless.
Again, motionless being the key.
Very few brain states involve us being mostly,
if not completely motionless, and yet awake.
And it turns out that brain state,
whether or not you call it yoga nidra,
you call it NSDR,
whether or not you call it meditation-induced
shift in consciousness as they did in this study,
although they do refer to yoga nidra,
all refer to the same thing,
which is being motionless and yet aware,
and relaxed, I should mention.
So in this study, what they did
was they brought subjects into the laboratory.
They had them either undergo
this self-directed deep relaxation
while they are motionless or mostly motionless,
or they had them listen to an audio script
while also just lying there with eyes closed.
And then they used a number of chemical tricks,
and I don’t want to get too deep into those now
because they can be a little bit distracting.
For those of you that are interested,
you can look at it in the study.
This is a binding of a chemical in the brain
that then they can image with brain imaging,
which is what they did in this study,
to evaluate how much dopamine changed in the brain
and where specifically in the brain
dopamine changed its levels before, during, and after
this particular behavioral practice
in one or the other group.
And what they discovered is that
people who did this deep relaxation,
that is self-directed deep relaxation,
lying there, eyes closed, relatively motionless,
although small movements of the body
or movements of the head are absolutely fine.
What they observed was a 65% increase in dopamine release.
Now here it’s key, dopamine release.
And they observed an increase in so-called theta activity.
Theta activity is a pattern of brainwave activity
that’s commonly associated with creative states
and divergent thinking in particular, so that’s important.
And they observed that across subjects,
specifically in the nigrostriatal pathway,
this pathway associated with divergent thinking.
So this is very exciting.
This is a study that really points to a behavioral tool
that can be used to selectively elevate dopamine
in the very pathway that one would want to
if they wanted to engage divergent thinking
for sake of creative exploration.
There are also a number of key observations
within this study.
First of all, the reduction in bodily movement
was essential.
In fact, when people rated
or when the amount of readiness for action in their system,
their body was evaluated,
what people found was that immediately after this practice,
they felt very still.
In other words, they felt as if remaining still was natural.
Now it’s not the case that they couldn’t move.
In fact, the elevation in dopamine that occurred
during this practice,
this yoga nidra-like non-sleep or NSDR-like practice
actually prepared them to be able to move
in a much more dedicated and robust way afterwards.
But during the practice,
their readiness for action went way, way down.
Not surprising, they were pretty much motionless.
But interestingly, as the level of readiness for movement
went down, down, down, down, down,
their degree of visual imagery,
that is their internal landscape
and their ability to imagine new things increased.
And in fact, areas of the brain
that are associated with visual imagery,
such as the visual or so-called occipital cortex
and the parietal cortex has been shown in other studies
to ramp up when people are motionless.
So there seems to be this inverse relationship
between movement and visual imagery, which makes sense.
When we’re moving,
we can pay attention to things in the outside world.
We tend to be aware of our sensory environment
to varying degrees,
but we don’t tend to be very focused on visual imagery
within our head.
Whereas when we lie down or sit down and close our eyes
and we are motionless,
the degree of visual imagery really increases.
Hence the increase in divergent thinking
because what essentially is happening
is the library of options,
the library of possible interactions
with whatever it is that you’re thinking about.
I give the example,
which is a trivial one on purpose of a pen,
but the bank of options that becomes available
when we are motionless
and when we are limiting our visualization
of the external world increases exponentially.
So this is important.
And what it points to is the fact
that this very simple,
completely non-pharmacologic behavioral practice
of lying down motionless for some period of time.
And I confess the amount of time
that they use in this study was quite long.
It was longer than 60 minutes,
but all the data that I’m aware of
in terms of NSDR and Yoganidra,
and there’s a growing body of literature
on these practices I should mention,
show that even 10 minutes
or even better would be 20 or 30 minutes
of lying motionless with eyes closed
and allowing the mind to drift wherever it happens to go,
but focusing on relaxing by doing long exhale breathing,
perhaps doing a body scan
of focusing your attention on particular body parts,
but not keeping it focused
on any one particular body part for that long.
That general practice of deep relaxation while awake
and being relatively motionless
really favors the brain states
associated with divergent thinking
and actually represents an accessing
of the various components
that you would use during divergent thinking.
And perhaps most excitingly,
it’s associated with this massive increase,
65% increase in dopamine release
within the very pathway that underlies divergent thinking.
So my recommendation would be for those of you
that are trying to enhance divergent thinking
and creative ability,
that you would do this practice at a minimum once per week.
And I should say, if you were going to do it once per week,
I’d recommend doing it for about 20 to 30 minutes.
Some of you might be able to do it
for as long as 60 minutes.
I myself do such a practice on a daily basis,
anywhere from 10 minutes to 20 minutes,
sometimes 30 minutes.
There’s an example of an NSDR script,
completely zero cost.
I confess it does happen to be my voice,
so forgive me in advance.
There are other options of NSDR.
You can go to YouTube, put NSDR and my name.
Again, completely zero cost.
You can get a sample
of what a 10 minute NSDR script looks like.
Through Virtusan, put that out there.
So thank you, Virtusan,
for putting that out there at zero cost.
There are examples of 20 and 30 minute NSDR scripts
and yoga nidra scripts.
Some that I particularly like.
We will also provide a link to some of those.
Again, those are completely zero cost for you to explore.
But more important than you follow any one particular
yoga nidra NSDR script
is that you learn to take your body and brain
into these states of limited motion,
elevated dopamine within this particular pathway,
and fairly deep relaxation.
Again, if you happen to fall asleep,
that’s not necessarily a bad thing.
Although the idea is that you stay in a shallow plane
of consciousness or sleep,
hence the term non-sleep deep rest.
So in any event,
I think this is a very useful practice
that many people could benefit from.
And the fact that it’s zero cost and purely behavioral,
I think adds additional benefit
because it’s certainly one that people could explore
depending on what amount of time you’re willing to commit.
And the research data on this
now extend beyond this one individual paper.
And I think is really exciting because what it says
is as the title and first line of the paper suggests
is that we can increase dopamine
using specific types of meditation induced consciousness.
And those increases in dopamine can be used
to increase our ability to be more creative.
Before moving forward,
I want to make absolutely clear
how it is that you would use NSDR,
AKA yoga nidra or similar.
The name doesn’t really matter after all,
the practice is what matters
in order to enhance dopamine in this nigrostriatal pathway
and enhance divergent thinking.
The key thing to understand here
is that the period of motionlessness
and deep relaxation while awake
increases dopamine in the nigrostriatal pathway.
It increases mental imagery.
That is, it increases access to the bank or the library,
if you will, of possible solutions
or elements to engage in the divergent thinking process.
But divergent thinking itself does not occur
during NSDR, AKA yoga nidra.
The NSDR and yoga nidra, deep relaxation, meditation,
whatever it is you want to call it,
sets a dopaminergic tone.
And that’s actually the appropriate use of the word,
dopaminergic tone.
It raises the baseline of dopamine transmission
in that circuitry that then positions you
to engage in divergent thinking more effectively.
So the idea would be to do anywhere from 10 to 20,
maybe 30 minutes, maybe even as much as an hour,
depending on how much time you had to dedicate
of such a meditation NSDR practice.
And then, not necessarily immediately,
but within the five to 15 minutes following,
then to go into a practice of divergent thinking
and start doing creative exploration.
That is to start thinking about different ways
to combine existing elements in whatever domain it is
that you want to achieve creativity.
So the point is that the divergent thinking itself
is not occurring during the NSDR or yoga nidra practice.
The NSDR and yoga nidra practice
prepares you for divergent thinking
that you do in the hour or hours that follows.
And just to contrast that with pharmacology,
I am not aware of any specific dopamine-related pharmacology
that would allow us to selectively increase dopamine
in the very pathway associated
with divergent thinking and creativity.
Now, there are forms of pharmacology
that can shift brain neurotransmitters and neuromodulators
in ways that favor creativity.
And this is certainly a topic
that we will go into in more depth in a future episode,
but there’s an exciting study
that was performed just this last year
looking at the role of serotonin, another neuromodulator,
in divergent and convergent thinking.
And it turns out that serotonin
underlies a lot of the brain activity
that’s responsible for both divergent
and for convergent thinking.
And there’s one particular form of pharmacology
which can enhance activation
of the serotonergic pathways associated
with the so-called 5-HT, that’s serotonin,
5-HT, that’s the abbreviation, 5-HT2A receptor,
serotonin 2A receptor in particular brain areas
in ways that favor both divergent and convergent thinking.
And the pharmacologic agent in that case
turns out to be very low dose,
or as some of you may have heard of it referred to
as microdosing of psilocybin.
Now I do want to say,
because it would be entirely inappropriate
for me to not say this,
that in most areas of the world,
and in particular in the United States,
psilocybin is still illegal.
It is not legal.
In some areas, it has been decriminalized.
And there are a number of different clinical trials
occurring now at Johns Hopkins, at Stanford,
at University of California, San Francisco,
and elsewhere exploring psilocybin
for the treatment of depression,
for trauma, for eating disorders.
Most of those studies focus on macro doses of psilocybin,
not microdosing.
There are far fewer studies of microdosing of psilocybin.
And I do have to point out that psilocybin use
and possession, and of course, sale is still illegal.
So I would be remiss if I didn’t state that.
However, I will provide a link to the study
that shows that microdosing of psilocybin
for a series of weeks on a daily basis.
So these are dosages of psilocybin
that do not induce hallucination
and do not massively shift mood or internal states
in any way that has people feeling like they are acting
or feeling that much different,
although some people do report a subjective shift,
does seem to increase divergent thinking ability.
But I do want to put a big asterisk,
a highlight and an underlying pen
beneath the statement I’m about to make,
which is that pharmacology of the serotonin system,
just as pharmacology of the dopamine system
is very broadband.
It’s a shotgun approach.
You’re going to hit all the circuits of the brain
that involve serotonin with microdosing psilocybin.
Although it has some selectivity for the 5-HT2A receptor,
it can attach to other receptors as well and act there.
This is the same reason why SSRI,
selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors,
can indeed shift mood and appetite,
but it can also shift libido and other things.
It’s because there are serotonin receptors everywhere,
or I should say many places,
not just in the areas of the brain
that are associated with mood, for instance.
And as I mentioned before,
agents, whether or not they are recreational
or illicit drugs or prescription drugs or supplements,
that increased dopamine will also be broadband
and hit a number of different circuits in parallel.
So this is why I always say
behavioral tools really should come first.
I don’t say that because I dislike pharmacology.
I say that because in many cases,
behavioral tools are not only safer and easier to titrate,
to adjust the duration, et cetera, than is pharmacology,
but also because they can sometimes,
as in the case of the study we just described,
afford you more specificity, not less, than pharmacology.
Pharmacology has its place, can be wonderful,
provided it’s safe and legal, et cetera,
but it can cause a lot of so-called off-target effects.
So for those of you that are interested
in increasing creativity through pharmacology,
I would say stay tuned for the data
on psilocybin and microdosing psilocybin.
If you are absolutely obsessed with the idea
of microdosing psilocybin for enhancing creativity
and you’d like to go straight to the study,
I will tell you what that study is
and therefore you can access some of the specifics
in terms of dosaging and protocols, et cetera.
So since I can’t help myself,
I’ll just very briefly summarize
that microdosing psychedelic study.
The title of the study, which was published in 2018,
is Exploring the Effect of Microdosing Psychedelics
on Creativity in an Open-Label Natural Setting.
Interesting title.
This was a microdosing event
organized by the Dutch Psychedelic Society.
They examined the effects of psychedelic truffles
where they knew what sorts of psychedelic compounds
were contained there on two creativity-related
problem-solving tasks, the picture concept task,
which I don’t expect you to recognize or know,
but it assesses convergent thinking
and the alternative uses task,
which I also don’t expect you to know,
but is a standard task for assessing divergent thinking.
They tested once before taking a microdose
and while the effects were expected to be manifested,
they say, oh, interesting.
They use the word manifested in a study of psychedelics.
Science is changing indeed.
In any case, what they found was an enhancement of creative,
that is divergent and convergent thinking,
not surprising given the fact that the 5-HT2A receptor
activity is increased by microdosing of psilocybin
and 5-HT2A receptors are present both on the neural circuits
that underlie divergent and convergent thinking.
So again, this is not a plug for microdosing psilocybin.
This is really in response to what I know
will be a number of different questions
about what sorts of pharmacologic agents
can be used to increase creativity.
So more on that later.
And again, we’ll provide a link
if you want to read that study in more depth.
I can imagine that a number of you
are probably also wondering about the effects of alcohol
and the effects of cannabis on creativity.
We did a long in-depth episode all about alcohol
and its effects on health.
The bottom line on alcohol
is that in excess of two drinks per week,
you’re starting to run into the cancer promoting
and toxic effects of alcohol.
I didn’t choose for the answer to be that,
but that’s what the data tell us.
Not telling you you can’t drink
more than two drinks per week.
I’m just saying that if you’re going to do that,
you should really consider offsetting that
with some other behavioral measures
all discussed in the episode on alcohol.
And despite what people think,
there is absolutely zero, zero evidence
that alcohol increases creativity.
However, by way of reducing activation
of the prefrontal cortex,
there is some evidence that alcohol and other substances
that reduce what it’s called autobiographical scripting,
that is a narrative about ourselves,
sort of self-awareness,
that it can enhance divergent thinking
at very low doses.
And this makes sense.
Divergent thinking involves remembering certain things
that we can use as elements in the creative process,
but suppressing narratives
about what the use of those would mean.
Will people like it?
Will they not like it?
Will it lead to the outcome we want?
Will it won’t?
All of that autobiographical scripting
involves the forebrain being very, very active
and specific regions of the forebrain in particular,
and that all needs to be suppressed,
which alcohol in very low doses can accomplish.
But again, that’s not a plug for alcohol.
I think behavioral tools would be a much better route,
but it therefore shouldn’t be surprising
why some people have used low-dose alcohol
in order to engage in the creative process
because it involves less inhibition or sense of self
that could be detrimental to the divergent thinking process.
Now, with respect to cannabis,
I went in depth into the biology
and the various uses, misuses, dangers,
and in some cases, benefits of cannabis use in certain,
the key word there is certain populations.
And I also dove into whether or not cannabis can be used
to increase divergent and convergent thinking.
So that’s timestamped in that episode.
I’ll refer you to that episode.
But the long and short of it is
that many of the ideas that people come up with
when under the influence of cannabis,
in particular high THC-containing cannabis,
does lead to enhanced divergent thinking,
but so enhanced, it turns out,
that oftentimes those ideas can’t be constrained
by the convergent thinking process.
In other words, they have lots of ideas that make sense
while under the influence of cannabis,
but that later cannot be implemented
into a coherent framework that leads
to any actual creative endeavor or creative product.
Or as is often the case with cannabis,
they simply can’t remember what they were thinking about.
Anytime there’s a discussion about dopamine,
there seems to be a discussion about motivation,
desire, and drive.
And of course, that makes sense given the roles of dopamine.
We did an entire episode on dopamine motivation and drive.
It’s one of our most popular episodes.
Again, you can access that with timestamps
in all formats at hubermanlab.com.
And anytime there’s a discussion
about dopamine and motivation,
we also seem to have a lot of questions
about attention and focus and ADHD
or attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in particular.
So just as a brief mention, there is a literature,
although not terribly extensive,
a small but strong literature on the relationship
between ADHD and creativity.
And the long and short of that literature
is that people who have ADHD regardless of age
do seem to have an ability to focus.
I’ve mentioned that in the episode on ADHD,
provided that they are interested
in the thing that they are focusing on.
So that runs counter to this idea
that people with ADHD simply can’t focus.
They can, but it tends to be a focus
that’s selective for things
that they are very excited about or interested in
as opposed to a general ability to focus.
What’s also highly underappreciated
is that people who have ADHD
oftentimes are very effective at divergent thinking,
but are less effective at convergent thinking.
What this tells us is that people with ADHD
can often have excellent novel and indeed creative ideas,
but that the implementation of those creative ideas
is sometimes challenged.
And that’s one reason to explore rational pharmacology,
nutrition, supplementation, et cetera.
Those are all things to explore in concert with,
or I should say in working closely
with a board certified physician
or ideally psychiatrist expert in ADHD.
You can also check out the episode that we did on ADHD.
There are a lot of tools there,
a lot of science mentioned there to support those tools.
Again, you can find that hubermanlab.com.
But I did think it was important to point out
even if briefly that having ADHD
is not a barrier to creativity.
In fact, may actually be an enhanced portal to creativity,
but that it doesn’t allow people
to access the convergent thinking
that allows creative ideas to be implemented
into specific strategies, pressure tested,
and eventually delivered in the form
of a final product of music, art, et cetera.
That is not to say that people with ADHD
cannot accomplish that,
but that it is going to require some additional steps
and protocols in order to enhance convergent thinking.
And that episode and the episode that we did on focus
and in particular tools to enhance focus
is very much directed at ways
to enhance convergent thinking.
So if you have ADHD or know somebody who does
and you’re interested in the creative process
or focusing generally,
please check out the episodes that I mentioned.
Now there’s also a small,
but nonetheless very exciting literature
on the relationship between physical movement
and divergent thinking.
This should come as no surprise to us.
As mentioned many times now in this episode,
the nigrostriatal pathway involved in divergent thinking
and it involves dopamine is also responsible for eye blinks
and for movements of the limbs of the body
in very deliberate ways.
This tells us that there’s some direct
or maybe indirect relationship
between movement of the body and divergent thinking.
And despite the fact that it’s only a few studies,
there have been some studies of whether or not
people are able to engage in divergent thinking
more effectively when they are doing things
like pacing or walking.
And this could be on a treadmill
or back and forth across the room.
And in fact, that is absolutely the case.
If you’re somebody like myself
who tends to have their best ideas,
not saying that my ideas are always terrific,
but among the ideas I have,
some of the better ones arrive to me
while on my long Sunday run.
I tend to do a long run or hike on Sundays,
sometimes with a light weight vest
or something of that sort.
But when I’m in a state of essentially
not directing my attention to any one thing
in my external environment,
this is extremely key for reasons
that now should be obvious.
Anytime we are directing our attention to a visual target
or an auditory target,
we are not as able to engage in divergent thinking.
This is why I will sometimes listen to podcasts
or to audio books while I go on these runs.
But for portions of these runs or hikes,
I tend to turn those off and just focus on the movement
and focus on not focusing on anything in particular.
And oftentimes I will stop and write down ideas
that suddenly or seemingly suddenly appear to me
or geyser to the surface.
I’ll have an idea.
Sometimes those are good ideas, sometimes less good ideas.
The fact that that happens for me
and the fact that many people are pacers or runners
or come up with their best ideas while in the shower
or while engaging in activities
that don’t require a lot of sensory attention
to one specific location,
either visual or auditory, et cetera,
that is because it engages these nigrostriatal pathways
through movement,
which then opens up this library of ideas
and allows the intersection of different ideas
that normally would be constrained to separate categories.
One way to think about this by analogy would be that,
you know, when I was a kid, you’d go to the library
and nowadays you just go online,
but the different pages of different books
on different topics are kept distinct from one another.
That is bound by different book covers and book ends,
different shelves in the library.
It’s as if different pages and elements from those books
are now being combined in a pseudo random,
not random, but in a pseudo random way.
And in that combination,
new possibilities about ways that information
could be combined and implemented start to arise.
So the tool that emerges from this is very simple
and it won’t necessarily apply to everybody.
But if you are somebody who finds that
just sitting in a chair and trying to be creative
is very challenging,
some of you might benefit from, for instance,
if you are engaging in writing or you want to write,
to talk into the voice recorder of your phone while walking
or simply walking and not attending
to any one specific thing visually or through headphones.
And then as ideas surface, seemingly out of nowhere,
which is how it happens,
that you could either put them into your phone
by voice dictation, or you could type them out if you like.
The key thing is to not be distracted
by other things in your phone,
not to start going onto social media or doing phone calls
or looking at text messages,
because that by definition is going to take you out
of this, what biologists call a pseudo random walk.
And this pseudo random element is extremely important.
We know, for instance,
that many circuits within the brain
have what’s called dedicated point-to-point wiring.
So for instance,
the brain circuits that govern your breathing,
the brain circuits that govern your heartbeat,
the brain circuits that govern your specific movements
once you are an adult
and allow for smooth directed movement are very precise,
very little slop, if any, in the wiring.
However, there are aspects of your brain circuitry,
yours and everybody else’s,
I should say, that are maintained into adulthood
that include a lot of extra wiring.
And these are fine wires,
they’re not the major highways
between different areas, if you will.
So sort of like Google Maps has highways and streets
and little passages and alleys,
but it’s as if there’s a little web
of additional possible pathways
cast over that entire thing.
The human brain maintains such webs of possible passage,
and it’s only during activities
such as walking, running, cycling,
swimming, hiking, pacing, et cetera,
that the activation of those pseudo-random pathways
starts to ramp up.
So this is a purely behavioral approach
to engaging different elements within neural networks
that normally would not communicate with one another
when we are completely still.
So again, the practices I talked about earlier
of being completely still to raise dopamine
and enhance divergent thinking,
those, I just want to reemphasize,
are designed to position you,
to ready you to engage in the kinds of activities
like walking and pacing, et cetera,
that best facilitate divergent thinking.
So if you are somebody
who wants to enhance divergent thinking,
I would encourage you to explore
how different patterns of movement,
in particular patterns of movement
that don’t require any conscious attention
to any one specific thing,
allow you to access new ideas
and new ways of combining existing elements
in whatever domain it is you want to be creative.
Now, this is also an opportunity
to underscore something I said back at the beginning,
which is you are not going to come up
with great works of music
if you don’t understand chords and melodies
and notes and music.
Those basic elements have to be built up
through some sort of formal
or at least rigorous or regular training,
in the same way that you’re not going to take a walk
and then suddenly be able to paint an incredible picture
if you have no painting ability.
That is not going to happen.
What I’m talking about here
are ways to enhance your capacity for divergent thinking,
such as NSDR,
and ways to engage in divergent thinking,
such as through certain forms of movement
that don’t require a lot of conscious attention
to your surroundings or any one specific sensory target.
And in doing so,
enhancing your ability to be more creative in a domain
for which you already have some degree of skill
or even mastery.
Now, in keeping with the theme
of how to enhance our creativity,
there’s a very exciting and yet parallel literature
to the literature that I’ve been describing thus far.
Now, I promise you that I’m not going to open up
an entire library of new information
related to neural circuits and so forth,
but I would be remiss if I didn’t mention
this parallel literature
because it speaks very specifically
to some important practices
that we can all use in order to enhance creativity
and to do so the first time and every time.
And this is really because certain scientists out there
have really gone through the trouble,
I should even say the painstaking trouble
of really trying to dissect what the creative process is,
both for individuals and in groups or even in pairs.
And so what I’m about to tell you
is beautifully encapsulated in an article
entitled, A New Method for Training Creativity,
Narrative as an Alternative to Divergent Thinking.
So again, we’ve been talking about divergent thinking,
that’s one pathway into the creative process,
but there are others as well.
And as it turns out, they’re not so distinct
in terms of the underlying brain mechanisms.
Nonetheless, let me describe briefly
how narrative can be used to train creativity
and to become more creative.
And in order to do that,
I’d like to just briefly paraphrase
or read from the first paragraph of this paper.
So what I’m about to read are the author’s words, not mine.
Quote, here’s a paradox.
According to current research,
young children are more imaginatively creative than adults.
And indeed that is true by the way.
Yet also according to current research,
creativity’s main neural engine is divergent thinking,
which relies on memory and logical association,
two tasks at which young children underperform adults.
That is, children are not as good
at divergent thinking as adults are.
So how could it be, the authors are asking,
that children are more imaginative
and thus more creative than adults?
This can only mean
that there are alternate pathways to creativity.
And indeed that is the case.
And so what this paper really explores
is other ways to access creativity.
And what they describe is what’s called narrative theory.
And there’s a number of different aspects
to this narrative theory,
but they agree that the standard definition of creativity
is the same one that we were talking about before.
So we’re not talking about a different form
of creativity here.
We’re talking about a different way to access creativity.
They describe the standard definition of creativity
as quote, the ability to generate novel ideas
that are useful.
So the commonly accepted one.
And what they cite as the basis for narrative theory
is this breakthrough finding in the 1950s.
This is the work of Guilford.
Some people out there might be familiar with it.
I was not at the outset of researching this episode.
What this theory from Guilford essentially states
is that there are different intellectual capacities
that are not captured by standard IQ tests.
I think that’s generally accepted nowadays.
We know there’s emotional intelligence.
We know there’s a standard IQ, et cetera.
But the important element to understand
is that these authors were able to trace back the idea
of narrative training as a way to enhance creativity
long before Guilford in the 1950s,
all the way back to Aristotle.
So this is incredible.
Narrative theory was actually birthed in 335 BCE
in his writing called Poetics,
which I think is incredible, at least to me,
that people long before us were thinking about creativity
and what goes into creativity.
And what Aristotle said, what Guilford then elaborated on
and what the authors of this paper further elaborate on
and actually have developed training protocols for
is the idea that there are three elements that we can use
in order to enhance creativity.
And those three elements are what’s called world building.
I’ll explain what these are in a moment.
Perspective shifting and action generating.
And right off the bat,
the word action should raise a flag for you.
And by that, I mean a positive flag
because once again, we are back into the world
and therefore the neural circuits of movement and motion.
Okay, so three elements of world building,
perspective shifting and active generating
are what make up this narrative approach to creativity.
And I should mention that these authors and others
are using such approach with companies,
with groups, with individuals.
So this is using a bunch of different contexts
to approach and enhance different forms of creativity.
So let’s talk first about world building techniques.
This is going to be immediately familiar to you
when you hear it.
But one of the key elements of creativity
is to, at the outset, come up with some idea
that makes sense or is attractive to you
about how the world is different
inside of your creative endeavor.
So for those that write science fiction
or think about science fiction,
there’s some obvious aspects to this.
But for those of you that don’t,
maybe you come up with a narrative, for instance,
in the context of storytelling that in your world,
we are the house cats,
and the cats are actually the ones
that are the curators of the earth.
Okay, so right there, there is a conceptual shift
that the world in which whatever creative idea
is going to emerge is entirely different
than the one that we actually live in.
So that sets a certain number of important constraints.
It means certain things are now possible.
Other things are not possible
that are very different from the world that we live in.
You can see the parallels here
to kind of childhood imagination,
where essentially anything can happen in the child’s mind
because they are unconstrained.
The second element is this perspective shifting techniques.
And the idea here is that not only are we supposed
to have the reader or the listener or the observer or us
explore for creativity and develop a creative idea
by thinking differently, right?
Which is kind of a generic term.
How do we actually think differently?
But rather than just say,
take the perspective of somebody else
in terms of what they would see or do or say or think,
rather, we are supposed to think
about their underlying motivation.
So we could do the world shift.
That is the world structure shift from step one.
And then in step two, you would ask yourself,
okay, rather than write about or think about
or move from the perspective of myself,
let’s say you’re feeling particularly happy that day.
You’d say, you know, I’m actually going to take
the perspective of somebody who’s angry,
but rather than just act angry,
I’m going to think about what their motivation
for being angry is.
Maybe they had a breakup.
Maybe they were jealous.
Maybe somebody had wronged them in some way.
Maybe they’re just generally angry at the world
for whatever reason,
and then operate from that motivational stance.
And this is a very interesting and powerful step
because what it really captures,
at least as viewed by me, the neuroscientist,
is it captures a whole set of neural circuits
about what that motivational state means
because motivational states dictate a huge number
of possible different outcomes,
but they really constrain the number
of different actions and outcomes
that any of us would engage in.
Rather than saying, I’m going to view the world
the way that someone else would view the world,
by stating that we are going to be motivated
by their set of motivations and not our own,
it includes a lot more possibilities
and yet not an infinite number of possibilities.
They are constrained in a logical way,
which is one of the key elements of creativity.
And then this third element,
which is action generating techniques,
is a really cool one that you will immediately notice
for the workplace, which is forced collaboration.
So inside of this thing that we’re building here,
this kind of story, you create a novel rule
for the world that your story is going to exist in,
or your music is going to exist in,
or your sport will exist in.
Then you create this perspective shift
where you take on the motivation
of someone else different than you.
And then you force collaboration between that person
who has this alternate motivation, different from you,
and someone else who has an entirely different motivation.
And in doing so, you create these kind of,
what are called the creative collisions.
Now they’re collisions because they’re crossing one another
and something new has to emerge from them.
They could be antagonistic,
they could be arguments fighting physical
or verbal or otherwise, they could be synergistic.
They could take on any number of different forms,
depending on the motivations
and the individuals that are involved.
But even though I just described this in fairly top contour,
what I just described is actually the core elements
of any story or any creative endeavor.
It’s just that many stories are from the perspective
of what we already know and believe
and think the world to be, and our own perspective,
and the actions that we would take
given that world and that perspective.
Whereas if we want to be creative,
we want to think outside of our usual framework
and yet using elements that exist within us, right?
No one has to tell us the creative narrative,
we’re trying to come up with it on our own.
We want to essentially think in a childlike way,
how do children think?
Well, they have new different or entirely novel concepts
about how the world works, but those are bounded.
And this is a key word, those are bounded.
They’re not infinite.
It’s not that anything can happen, right?
Some kids will say we can fly
and you can shoot lasers out of your eyes.
You can do all sorts of things.
There’s unicorns, a candy falling from the sky.
At some point, if you don’t bound the change in the world,
it just becomes pure chaos.
And even children don’t do that.
So we need to bound the change
and yet create some alternate universe, if you will,
in which the story takes place or the creation of any kind,
doesn’t have to be a story takes place.
Then there has to be a perspective shift.
And this is very useful.
This is actually a tool that we can all use
of trying to take the perspective of others,
but not just asking what they would feel or think or do,
but ask what is their motivation in life generally?
Or what kind of mood stance or goal stance are they taking?
Are they trying to extract from others?
Are they trying to give to others?
Are they very altruistic, et cetera, et cetera.
And then you take that individual
and you do that also for another individual
or group of individuals.
And you start thinking about how those different individuals
because of their different motivational states
would engage at the level of action, what they would do,
what they would say, would they mate, would they fight,
would they, et cetera, et cetera.
You think of any story, the story of Star Wars,
the Greek myths, you think of any story
that has been created,
which we consider great and novel works.
And you start to find these three elements,
world-building, perspective-shifting
and action-generating techniques.
And so, while this is again,
just a broad contour of what this narrative approach
involves, I think it’s a very important
and very exciting one because it gives us a formula, right?
We already know that divergent thinking
and convergent thinking are both elements
to the creative process.
This is suggesting that whether or not it involves
divergent thinking or not,
and these authors seem to think this is distinct
from divergent thinking,
that capturing some of the elements of creativity
that are present in childhood,
but that then tend to disappear
as we start to assume identity, build identity
and understand rules about the actual world we live in,
all of those basic elements of early childhood creativity
can be reawakened.
And in fact, they have data to support the fact
that they can be reawakened in adults in meaningful ways
that can lead to new product design,
new workplace interactions and on and on.
That I find very exciting.
And as a consequence, I do intend to do an entire episode
at some point on narrative and storytelling
and the role of narrative and storytelling,
not just for sake of creativity,
but also for accessing neuroplasticity
and for enhancing memory and so on.
There’s an entire landscape of literature
and exciting tools and things to understand there.
But in the meantime, we will provide a link to this paper.
And for those of you that choose not to access the paper,
simply understanding these three aspects of narrative
as an alternative to accessing creativity,
that is a dedicated and well understood
or established world shift that you choose,
perspective shifting and taking on the motivation of others
and creating some sort of landscape of exploration
for what sorts of interactions would occur
between that individual or groups of individuals
and other individuals that have other motivations
and yet are still living in this alternate world.
Those three elements we now know can be combined
into what you or I or anyone
would consider important creative works.
So today we discussed creativity,
this absolutely fascinating aspect to human brain function
that has allowed us as a species to develop everything
from great works of art and music
to technological innovations that allow us to fly
and allow us to access people all over the world
through little screen devices
that we carry around in our pockets and on and on.
As I mentioned at the beginning of today’s episode,
I find creativity to be one of the most fascinating aspects
of brain function and in particular,
because we don’t actually know
what the upper limits of creativity are
and yet we understand that there are certain bounds,
there are certain requirements
and the key requirement for creativity
is this aspect of utility.
Now that doesn’t necessarily mean
that for something to be considered creative,
it has to be useful in the practical sense,
but it does seem that for something
to be considered truly creative
or especially creative in some cases,
that it revealed to us something fundamental
about the way that we or the world works.
We discussed some of the neural circuits
that underlie the different aspects of creativity
in particular divergent and convergent thinking
as well as narrative building
and some of the tools and steps
that can allow us to better access
divergent thinking and convergent thinking
and those tools include behavioral tools
as well as pharmacology
and we talked about narrative building
as a way to reawaken or I should say,
reaccess the childhood creativity
that did indeed exist in all of us at some point in time.
If you’re learning from and or enjoying this podcast,
please subscribe to our YouTube channel.
That’s a terrific zero cost way to support us.
In addition, please subscribe to the podcast
on Spotify and Apple and on both Spotify and Apple,
you can leave us up to a five-star review.
If you have questions for us or comments about the podcast
or suggestions about guests
that you’d like us to interview on the Huberman Lab Podcast,
please put those in the comment section on YouTube.
We do read all the comments.
Please also check out the sponsors mentioned
at the beginning and at various times
during today’s episode.
That’s the best way to support this podcast.
As mentioned at the beginning of today’s podcast,
we’ve partnered with Momentous Supplements
because Momentous Supplements are the very highest quality.
They have single ingredient formulations
which turns out to be most effective
in building a supplement protocol
and when I say most effective,
I mean most biologically effective
and most cost effective.
And we’re fortunate that they ship internationally
because we know that many of you reside
outside of the United States.
If you want to check out the supplements mentioned in today’s
and in other episodes of the Huberman Lab Podcast,
supplements that are aimed at enhancing sleep,
enhancing focus, hormone support, et cetera,
you can go to Live Momentous spelled O-U-S,
so it’s livemomentous.com slash Huberman.
If you haven’t already subscribed
to the Huberman Lab Podcast neural network newsletter,
this is a monthly newsletter that is completely zero cost
that has summaries of particular podcast episodes
as well as toolkits for things like enhancing sleep,
enhancing focus, dopamine, deliberate cold exposure,
and many other topics important to the listeners
of the Huberman Lab Podcast.
If you’d like to join the newsletter,
please go to HubermanLab.com,
click on the menu and go to newsletter.
You provide your email to us.
We do not share your email with anybody else
and as I already mentioned, it is completely zero cost.
If you’re not already following us on Instagram,
Twitter, Facebook, and or LinkedIn, please do so.
It is Huberman Lab on all platforms.
On all those social media platforms,
I describe science and science-related tools,
some of which overlap with the content
of the Huberman Lab Podcast,
but often which does not overlap
with the content of the Huberman Lab Podcast.
Again, it’s Huberman Lab on all platforms.
So thank you once again for joining me
for today’s discussion,
all about the science and tools for creativity.
And last but certainly not least,
thank you for your interest in science.