Lex Fridman Podcast - #274 - Karl Deisseroth: Depression, Schizophrenia, and Psychiatry

Where are the darkest places you’ve ever gone in your life?

The following is a conversation with Karl Deisseroth,

professor of bioengineering, psychiatry,

and behavioral sciences at Stanford University.

He’s one of the greatest living psychiatrists

and neuroscientists in the world.

He’s also just a fascinating human being.

We discuss both the darkest and the most beautiful places

that the human mind can take us.

He explores this in his book called

Projections, A Story of Human Emotions.

I highly recommend it.

It’s written masterfully.

This is the Lex Friedman podcast.

To support it, please check out our sponsors

in the description.

And now, dear friends, here’s Karl Deisseroth.

You open your book called Projections,

A Story of Human Emotions with a few beautiful words

to summarize all of humanity.

The book draws insights about the human mind

from modern psychiatry and neuroscience.

So if it’s okay, let me read a few sentences

from the opening.

You gotta give props to beautiful writing when I see it.

Quote, in the art of weaving, warp threads are structural

and strong and anchored at the origin,

creating a frame for crossing fibers

as the fabric is woven.

Projecting across the advancing edge into free space,

warp threads bridge the formed past

to the ragged present to the yet featureless future.

Yet featureless future, well done, well done, sir.

The tapestry of the human story has its own warp threads,

rooted deep in the gorges of East Africa,

connecting the shifting textures of human life

over millions of years, spanning pictographs

backdroped by crevice ice, by angulated forestry,

by stone and steel, and by glowing rare earths.

The inner workings of the mind give form to these threads,

creating a framework within us,

upon which the story of each individual can come into being.

Personal grain and color arise from the crossed threads

of our moments and experiences, the fine weft of life,

embedding and obscuring the underlying scaffold

with intricate and sometimes lovely detail.

Here are stories of this fabric fraying in those who are ill.

In the minds of people for whom the warp

is exposed and raw and revealing.

What have you learned about human beings,

human nature and the human mind,

from those who suffer from psychiatric maladies,

for those for whom this fabric is warped?

And one thing we learn as biologists

is that when something breaks,

you see what the original unbroken part was for.

And we see this in genetics, we see this in biochemistry.

It’s known that when you have a mutated gene,

sometimes the gene is turned up in strength

or turned down in strength,

and that lets you see what it was originally for.

You can infer true function from dysfunction.

And this is a theme that I thought needed to be shared

and needed to be made communicable

to the lay public, to everybody.

People who, which is, I think, almost all of us,

who think and care about the inner workings of our mind,

but who also care for those who have been suffering,

who have mental health disorders, who face challenges.

But then more broadly,

it’s a very much larger story than the present.

There’s a story to be told

where the protagonist really is the human mind.

And that was one thing I wanted to share as well

in projections, is that broader story,

but still anchored in the moment of patients,

of people, of experiences of the moment.

Is there a clear line between dysfunction and function,

disorder and order?

This is always debated in psychiatry,

probably more so than any other medical specialty.

I’m a psychiatrist, I treat patients still.

I see acutely ill people who come to the emergency room

where there’s no doubt that this is not something

that’s working well, where the manifestation of disease

is so powerful, where the person is suffering so greatly,

where they cannot continue as they are.

But of course, it’s a spectrum,

and there are people who are closer

to the realm of being able to work okay in their jobs,

but suffer from some small dysfunction.

And everywhere in between.

In psychiatry, we’re careful to say

we don’t call it a disease or a disorder

unless there’s a disruption

in social or occupational functioning.

But of course, psychiatry has a long way to go

in terms of developing quantitative tests.

We don’t have blood draws, we don’t have imaging studies

that we can use to diagnose.

And so that line, ultimately, that you’re asking about

between order and disorder, function and dysfunction,

it’s operational at the moment.

How are things working?

Can we just linger on the terms for a second?

So this disease, dysfunction,

how careful should we be using those words?

Can we just, even in this conversation,

from a sort of technical perspective,

but also a human perspective,

how quick should we be

in saying that schizophrenia, depression, autism,

as we kind of go down across the spectrum

of different maladies,

to use the word dysfunction and disease?

I would say to give ourselves license

to capture the whole spectrum, let’s say disorder,

because that captures truly, I think, the essence of it,

which is we need to talk about it when it’s not working,

when there’s disorder.

And that’s the fairest and most inclusive term to use.

So is it fair to assume that basically every member

of the human species suffers from a large number

of disorders then?

Well, we just have to pick which ones are debilitating

for each person?

You know, if you look at the numbers,

there are, if you look at how our mental health disorders

are currently defined,

you can look at population prevalence values

for all these disorders,

and you can come up with estimates

that somebody will have a lifetime prevalence

of having a psychiatric disorder

that approaches 25% or so.

And so that’s, and in some studies it could be more,

some studies it could be less.

Now, what do we do with that number?

What does that mean?

And in some ways, that’s the essence

of what I was hoping to approach with the book,

is to reflect on this spectrum that exists

for all the disorders.

There is, and taking nothing away from the severity

and the suffering that comes at the extreme end

of these illnesses, but nearly every one of them exists

on a spectrum of severity, from nearly functional

to completely dysfunctional, life threatening,

and even fatal.

And so that number, 25%, more or less,

it doesn’t capture that spectrum of severity.

To look at that number, where do those numbers come from?

Is it self report?

Is it people who show up and say, I need help?

Is it somebody else that points out that person needs help?

Or is it like estimates that even go beyond that

for people who don’t ask for help

or are suffering quietly alone?

When you look at self report numbers,

then those numbers get even higher, beyond 25% or more.

Those, the most rigorous studies are done

with structured psychiatric interviews

where people who are trained in eliciting symptoms carefully

do complete psychiatric inventories of individuals.

And these are time consuming laborious studies

that are not often repeated.

When they’re done, they’re done well.

But very often you’ll see a report or something

in the news of a very high number

for some disorder or symptom.

And very often, if it’s shockingly high,

that’s coming from a self report of a person.

And so that’s another issue that we have, again,

take nothing away from the severity and reality

and biological nature of these disorders,

which are very genetic, very, you know,

we understand that these are very biological.

And yet, we lack right now the lab tests

and the blood draws to make the diagnoses.

We’ll talk about it, just how biological they are,

because that too is a mystery.

You know, in terms of from our perspective

of how to probe into the disease,

how to understand it, how to help it.

So some of it could be neurobiological,

some of it could be just the dance

of human emotion and interaction.

And it’s like, is love when it works

and is love when it breaks down biological

or is it something else?

So we’re gonna talk about it.

But let me just like to linger in terms of disorder.

What about genius?

You know, that sort of cliche saying,

like the madness and genius

that they kind of dance together.

What about if the thing we see as disorder

is actually genius, unheard or misunderstood?

Well, here again, the numbers help us.

And here’s where being rigorous

and quantitative actually really helps.

If you look at disorders like autism

and bipolar disorder and eating disorders,

anorexia nervosa, for example,

these, you know, particularly bipolar and anorexia,

these can be fatal, they can cause immense suffering,

but they are heavily genetic, all three of these.

And what’s very interesting is each one of these three

is actually correlated positively,

positively with measures of intelligence,

of educational attainment, and even of income.

And so you look at this severe disorders

in many cases causing quite an immense morbidity

and mortality, and yet they are positively correlated

at the population level with positive things.

Can you say the three again, autism?

Autism, anorexia, and bipolar disorder.

Bipolar, right.

What’s that book, forgot the book name,

but is intelligence a burden?

Well, you know, people can get into trouble

when they think they’re smarter than they are,

I will say that.

I don’t know.

Sometimes, like, in the deepest meaning of that statement,

I think ignorance is bliss.

I’m a big fan of Prince Mishkin from The Idiot

and Aliosha from Brother Karamazov.

Optimism can be seen as naivety and dumbness,

but I think it’s a kind of deep intelligence.

Maybe inability to reason sort of about the mechanics

of the world, but instead kind of feel the world.

It seems like that’s one of the paths to happiness.

There is.

How much you think versus how much you feel,

this comes up all the time.

In medicine, we encounter this all the time.

Day after day, you encounter the abyss of suffering

from patients.

How much do you let yourself feel?

Or how much do you make it abstract and objective

and try to make it clinical?

And that range, how you’re able to move yourself

on that spectrum, is very important for survival

as a physician, and the way you protect yourself

and your feelings turns out to be very important.

You quote Finnegan’s Wake, mad props for that,

James Joyce book.

I took a class in James Joyce in college.

I think I read parts of Finnegan’s Wake.

I might have been on drugs of some kind,

or I somehow got an A in that class,

which probably refers to some kind of curve

where nobody understood anything.

The only thing I understood and really enjoyed

is his short stories, The Dead, and then Ulysses.

I kind of, I think, read a few Cliff Notes

that kind of got to the point,

and then Finnegan’s Wake was just a hopeless.

For people who haven’t looked at it,

maybe you can elucidate to me better,

but I felt like I was reading things, words,

and the words made sense, like standing next to each other,

but when you kind of read for a while,

you realize you didn’t actually understand

anything that was said.

Right, but did you have a feeling, though?

That’s one thing I found interesting about Finnegan’s Wake.

I never fully understood it,

but the words caused feelings in me,

which I found fascinating,

and sometimes I couldn’t predict it

from the semantic black and white context

of what I was seeing in front of me on the page,

but the rhythm or the melody

would make me feel certain ways,

and that was what I always was intrigued by with Joyce.

Of course, that was his, he existed on a spectrum, too,

and he wrote, as you say, more accessible works.

I learned a lot about Irish history

from Portrait of the Artist as a Young Man,

and there was just, he could be as objective

as he wanted to be, but then when he let himself loose,

he was in this realm where the words

had their own purpose separate from semantic meaning

from their dry dictionary definition.

You know, there’s a funny story that was told,

doesn’t matter if it’s true or not,

but they said that James Joyce, when he was young,

when he was in his teen years,

would go around sort of Ireland drinking and so on

and telling everybody that he’s going to be one of,

if not the greatest writers of the 20th century,

and he turned out to be that.

So I always think about that little story

that somebody told me,

because I have a lot of people come up to me,

including myself, I’m a bit of a dreamer.

You get into certain moods where you say

I’m going to be the greatest anything ever.

You get people tell you this, especially young people,

and it kind of, it makes me feel all kinds of ways,

but that story reminds me that you just might be

one of the greatest writers of the 21st century,

for example, if somebody were to tell me that,

and don’t immediately disregard that,

because one of the people that say that,

that’s almost like a precondition,

that’s a good requirement just to believe in yourself.

Maybe it’s not a full requirement,

but it’s an interesting story.

I think when someone tells you that,

then it creates, one sees an opportunity,

and then it would be a tragedy

if the opportunity weren’t captured, right?

And so then that creates some impetus,

some motivation to do something good.

I think the mind, it’s like, I guess that’s what

books or whatever, I don’t even know if it’s a book,

The Secret plugs into, they kind of make

a whole industry out of it.

But there is something about the mind

believing something, making it a reality.

It is just time and time again with Steve Jobs,

your belief in yourself, your belief in an idea,

sort of embracing the me versus the world,

embracing the madness of this idea

and making it a life pursuit,

somehow morphs reality around you

for some tiny fraction of the population.

For everybody else, you descend into

all the beautiful ways that failure

materializes in our lifetime.

Well, you know, you mentioned love earlier.

I mean, that’s a great example of how

belief in something makes it real, right?

It’s not reasonable on the face of it,

but because you believe it’s reasonable,

then it actually does become reasonable,

and then it is real.

And so that’s a good example.

That doesn’t happen.

I’m also in a bioengineering department.

We don’t imagine that a bridge is soundly built

and then it is soundly built.

That’s something that, it doesn’t come up

in too many realms of human existence,

but love is one of them.

And the ability to have a fixed idea

and to say it’s true, and then it is true.

A bridge is a kind of manifestation of love.

So maybe it does work a little bit,

but it can break down like Chernobyl did.

You can’t just say it’s safe.

You have to also prove it’s safe.

But on Finnegan’s Wake, I think,

maybe correct me if I’m wrong,

you’re using kind of Finnegan’s Wake

to give one perspective on what madness is,

of what’s going on in the mind.

How much of that is that we’re simply unable

to communicate with the person

on the other side of their mind?

Like there’s almost like a little person inside the brain

and they have some circuitry that’s used

to communicate emotion, communicate ideas

to the outside world.

And there’s something about that circuitry

that makes it difficult to communicate

with the little person on the other side.

So if you look at what shows up in schizophrenia,

with many cases, what we call thought disorders,

what we call the individual speech symptoms

of schizophrenia, Finnegan’s Wake is loaded with them.

And it’s just full of them.

We talk about clang associations in schizophrenia

where the word that is said echoes in some way

the previous word.

And we call that a clang association

because there’s no other reason

than the similarity of the sound,

like a clang of a garage door being hit.

And it has a, and sometimes it’s not even a word,

and we call that a neologism, a new word being created.

And of course, Finnegan’s Wake is full of that.

And then we also, in schizophrenia,

where there’s what we call loose associations

or tangential thought processes,

of course, full of that where things just go off

in directions that are not linear or logical.

And you can’t read Finnegan’s Wake, I think,

without, certainly as a psychiatrist,

you can’t read it without thinking about schizophrenia.

And then when we look at the families of people

with schizophrenia, and Joyce was no exception,

there very often are people within the family

who are on the spectrum.

Some have it.

Some are able to see it from a distance,

from a safe distance.

There’s an association between schizophrenia

and what we call schizotypal personality disorder

where people are not quite in this severe state

of schizophrenia but have some magical thinking,

have some unusual thought patterns.

Very often, those are family members

of people with schizophrenia.

So this points to this, again, to this idea

that there is a range, even along this very severe,

very genetic biological illness that human beings dwell

on different spots along that spectrum.

I should mention that we have my friend, Sergey,

pulling up stuff, young Sergey or old Sergey,

I don’t know what to call you,

but there’s drafts of Finnegan’s Wake.

Yeah, I actually saw pictures of this from,

I think it was on Instagram or something.

These are early drafts of Finnegan’s Wake.

And it’s so beautiful to see.

For people who are just listening,

there’s just random paragraphs and writing

all over the page with stuff crossed out.

And it’s great to see that Joyce himself

was thinking in this kind of way

as you’re putting it together.

How much do you think he was thinking

about the schizophrenic mind?

I think a lot.

I think it’s known that his daughter suffered

from schizophrenia.

And this is, what’s depicted here on the page

is something that I’m sure he either felt himself

in some level was able to access

this nonlinearity of processing

or had seen enough in family that he knew what it was

and was able to reflect it down

in black and white on the paper.

So what he was able to do was quite authentic

in that sense.

Of course, I don’t want to pigeonhole him.

He was doing much more than that.

It was much more than talking about altered human

thought processes and thought disorders.

But that was an aspect that he was so good at representing

that it had to be intentional to some extent.

And a tiny tangent.

What does your own writing look like for this book?

Because it’s extremely well written.

How many edits?

Did you just drink some whiskey

and like I’m imagining Hemingway style?

What’s a very different, the writing is very different.

I mean, it’s really, really well written,

which was like, I was reading it.

It makes you realize,

because I was expecting sort of a science kind of,

which it is like elucidating something

about the human mind kind of thing.

But you could also probably write really strong novels.

So maybe that’s in the future.

But anyway, what is your, how many edits?

What’s your style?

Does it look like that?

Is it more structured, organized?

Unfortunately, I used a laptop,

so I didn’t have this sort of a beautiful record.

No typewriters, cigarette, and whiskey.

I did explore which was their particular altered state

that would help me to be most creative.

And I found actually, I actually did the best

while sober, but slightly disinhibited

in the late hours of the night or early morning.

Yeah, particularly late hours of the night there.

I have a friend who would tell me

that she thought that very early in the morning,

her inner critic was still asleep

and she could write more effectively

before her inner critic woke up.

And I actually found that outstanding advice for me

that I often found that I was looser

and could write more in the morning.

But the other interesting thing is each chapter,

each story, it’s about a different human being

with a different class of psychiatric disorder.

That’s what each story, each chapter is anchored in.

But I’m trying to use words and style of writing

and diction that captures the feeling of the disorder.

And so it’s different in each story.

In the story about mania, which is a very expansive,

exuberant, at least briefly uplifting state

where the words come out in a torrent

and they’re complex and pressured and elaborate.

I try to capture that feeling

with the words used in that chapter.

And then in the schizophrenia or psychosis chapter,

where things slowly fragment over time

and become looser and separated,

I try to capture that in the writing too.

So for each, it wasn’t as if there was a single mode

I could be in for the whole book.

For each chapter, I had to put myself into a different mode

to capture that inner feeling of the disorder.

When you put yourself in that mode, does that change you?

Yeah, I couldn’t turn it on and off right away.

I had to, first I would start by thinking about the person

or the people, one or two people based on real patients

and the stories that are put forth.

The symptom descriptions are real,

they’re from the patients.

Of course, all details change to protect privacy,

but the actual symptom descriptions are real.

And I would sit with them and really try to inhabit

the space of the mind of that person that I knew.

And that’s not instantaneous.

It would take some time.

I needed quiet, I needed to be still.

That’s another reason late at night is good.

Sergey posted that drowsiness gives creativity boosts,

according to Andrew Huberman.

Thank you, Andrew.

He’s not wrong, he’s not wrong.

Why projections?

Is it, I mean, instead of putting words into your mouth,

because I can imagine a lot.

I mean, to me, I will start putting words in your mouth,

despite what I just said.

So, I mean, to me, projections,

working on neural networks, for example,

from an artificial neural networks,

from a machine learning perspective,

it’s often, that’s exactly what you’re doing.

You have an incredibly complex thing

and you’re trying to find simple representations

in order for you to make sense of it.

So I was kind of thinking about in that way,

which is like this incredibly complex neural network

that is kind of projecting itself onto the world

through this low bandwidth expression of emotion and speech

and all that kind of stuff.

And the way it’s, we only have that window into your soul,

the eyes and the speech and so on.

So in that way, when there’s any kind of disorder,

we get to only see that disorder through that narrow window

as opposed to the full complexity of its origins.

The word projections definitely serves that purpose here,

but it’s got a few other really appropriate

other connotations as well.

So the first thing is a projection

in terms of neuroscience is this long range connection

that goes from one part of the brain to another.

And so it’s what binds two parts of our brain together.

There are projections, long range connections

of axons, these are the outgoing threads

that connect one part of the brain to another part.

There’s a projection that links, for example,

auditory cortex where we hear things to reward centers

where we can feel, where feelings of pleasure

and reward are initiated.

And it’s been shown that if you have reduced connectivity

along that dimension, you are less able to enjoy music.

And so these connections, these projections matter.

They define how effectively two parts of the brain

can engage with each other and join together

to form a joint representation of something.

So that’s one meaning, it’s pure neuroscience.

The word projection is used all the time.

And it happens to be something that optogenetics,

a technique that maybe we’ll talk about a little later

that works particularly well with,

we can use light to turn on or off the activity

along these projections from one spot of the brain

to another.

And this is particularly referring

to the long range connections.

It’s particularly straightforward

along these long range projections

that connect different parts of the brain,

but it works over shorter range too.

But then there’s this other meaning of projections

which you were bringing up, which is very relevant,

which is at some point you can reduce something

from one level of dimensionality to another,

and you can project down into a lower dimensional space,

for example.

And then finally, there’s a psychiatric term projections

which comes up all the time, which is we very often

will look at our internal states

and to understand somebody else,

we’ll project them onto somebody else.

We’ll try to understand someone else’s behavior

and make sense of it by projecting our own inner feelings,

our own sort of narrative onto them

and use that as a way to help us understand them better.

And we’ll do the reverse too.

We’ll take things we see in the outside world

and we’ll bring them into ourselves

and see how well they map, how well they align.

That’s called introjection.

So projections turns out to be a really rich word.

And then finally, of course, there’s the very common

sense of it as a projector that illuminates

by conveying information across space with light.

So for English, for English language,

perfect word to use for this book.

But what’s funny is not every,

there are a lot of international translations now

and all those rich connotations

aren’t captured in other languages.

And so for some translations, connections is used

instead of projections.

In fact, even in England,

the British version is connections instead of projections

because apparently projections

doesn’t have the full connotation, I was told.

So you have to sacrifice some of the rich ambiguity

of meaning with connections, that’s interesting.

I mean, connect, man, words are so interesting.

They have so many meaning.

I love language and how much is lost in translation.

I’m very fortunate enough to be able to speak.

I’m not good at languages.

I was just, I guess, forced to by life’s circumstance

to learn two languages, Russian and English.

And it’s just so interesting to watch

how much of culture, how much of people,

how much of history is lost in translation.

The poetry, the music, the history, the pain,

the way the scientists actually express themselves,

which is funny.

I mean, it’s so sad to see how much brilliant work

that was written in Russian.

There’s a whole culture of science in the Soviet Union

that is now lost.

It makes me wonder, in the modern day,

how much incredible science is going on in China

that is lost in translation.

And I’ll never, I mean, that makes me very sad

because I’ll never learn Chinese in the same way

that I’ve learned English and Russian.

Maybe, whenever I say stuff like that,

people are like, well, there’s still time.

But, you know, yeah, that’s actually fair,

that I think the 21st century, both China and U.S.

will have very important roles in the scientific development

and we should actually bridge the gap through language.

And that doesn’t just mean convincing Chinese

to speak English.

That means also learning Chinese.

Well, we need these bridge people who can do both.

You know, Nabokov, for example, writing in English

beautifully, one of my favorite poets, Borges,

who I mentioned earlier, he wrote both in English

and in Spanish, I think beautifully in both.

We need those people who can serve as bridges

across cultures who really can do both.

You mentioned Borges, so you open your book

with a few lines from a poem by Jorge Luis Borges,

a love poem.

I’m gonna read parts of it because it’s a damn good poem.

It’s called Two English Poems.

I mean, there’s, I’d like to understand why you used it

and the specific parts you used, which is interesting.

But then when I read the full thing,

so I think you used it as a sort of beautiful description

of what it means to delve deep into understanding,

offering yourself to the task of understanding

another human being.

But if you look at the full context of the poem,

it’s also a damn good description of being hit by love

and overtaken by it and sort of,

and trying to figure out how to make sense of the world

now that you’ve been stricken by it.

It says a bunch of things about chatting

and significantly with friends and all those kinds of things

and then the poem reads, the big wave brought you.

I get, this is the moment, I guess, of the universe

where the two people you fall in love.

Maybe I’m totally misreading this poem, by the way.

Doesn’t matter, you can’t misread a poem.

So it goes on, words, any words, your laughter,

and you’re so lazily and incessantly beautiful.

We talked and you have forgotten the words.

The shattering dawn finds me in a deserted street

of my city, your profile turned away.

The sounds that go to make your name.

The lilt of your laughter.

These are the illustrious toys you have left me.

So these little memories of these peculiar little details,

he remembers, those are the illustrious toys.

I apologize to mix my own words with the poem,

but you should definitely read it.

I turn them over in the dawn, I lose them.

I find them.

I tell them to the few stray dogs

and to the few stray stars of the dawn.

Your dark, rich life.

I must get at you somehow.

I put away those illustrious toys you have left me.

I want your hidden look, your real smile.

That lonely, mocking smile your cool mirror knows.

I want your hidden look, your real smile.

So this is the first part of the poem,

and then it goes on, which is some of the parts

that you reference.

Second part is, what can I hold you with?

I offer you lean streets, desperate sunsets,

the moon of the jagged suburbs.

I offer you the bitterness of a man

who has looked long and long at a lonely moon.

I offer you my ancestors, my dead men,

the ghosts that living men have honored in bronze.

My father’s father killed in the frontier of Buenos Aires,

two bullets through his lungs, and so on, so on.

I offer you whatever insights my books may hold,

whatever manliness or humor in my life.

I offer you the loyalty of a man who has never been loyal.

I offer you that carnival of myself

that I have saved somehow, the central heart

that deals not in words, traffics, not with dreams,

and is untouched by time, my joy, and adversities.

And I think this is the part that you include in the book.

I offer you the memory of a yellow rose seen at sunset,

years before you were born, I, damn, that’s a good line.

Okay, I offer you explanations of yourself,

theories about yourself,

authentic and surprising news of yourself.

I can give you my loneliness, my darkness,

the hunger of my heart.

I’m trying to bribe you with uncertainty,

with danger, with defeat.

That is a man who’s in love and longing.

If I’ve taken, but I just wanna go back to,

maybe you could say why you wanted to include that poem,

but also your dark, rich life, I must get at you somehow.

I put away those illustrious toys you have left me at.

I want your hidden look, your real smile,

that lonely, mocking smile, your cool mirror nose.

Sometimes I meet a stranger, and I just,

it’s like a double take.

It’s like, who are you?

Have we met before somewhere?

Who’s that person behind there?

And I wanna get at that, whatever that is.

And of course, maybe that’s what love is,

because maybe that’s the whole pursuit,

like a lifelong pursuit of getting at that person.

Maybe that’s what that is,

and like that insatiable sort of curiosity to keep getting.

Like, well, who is that person in your own private life?

Yeah, so that, absolutely, I think that,

it was a beautiful description of what you just said,

when there’s that first moment,

and then you wanna dive deeper.

You want to know what the hidden mysteries are.

In a way, it’s a love poem.

As a scientist, though, it also,

it’s a bit of how a scientist can love science,

and that wanting to dive deeper is,

it’s almost like, again, where the,

it could be a love affair

with investigating the human mind, for example.

And that was one reason it spoke to me also.

Again, thinking about the broader sweep

of where the human mind came from,

and the steps it took to get where it is today,

what was given up along the way,

what compromises were made.

And here’s where the darkness of the poem

starts to come in a little bit, too.

It doesn’t shy away from the negativity,

from the confusion, from the danger.

And then at the very end,

the boardface is offering up scenes from his life,

parts of himself, and this is how we connect with people.

We offer up parts of ourselves, just here it is,

and then we see how well does that map onto what you have.

And it’s that offering up that I liked,

and not the good stuff, or not only the good stuff.

The yellow rose is nice,

but he’s offering up the bad stuff, too.

And that, to me, was important for the book,

because I’m offering up hard stuff, too.

In fact, a lot of it.

And also, hard stuff from within me,

from my own personal side, too.

And that was, there’s a lot of vulnerability

that comes with that, but that comes with love,

that comes with writing.

You have to be open, you have to be vulnerable.

And so, I thought that reflected what I was trying to do,

and I thought it was, as an epigraph,

it kind of made it clear how vulnerable I was

in taking this step, but also what could come out of it.

And also, in a meta way,

because I was not familiar with this poem,

it made me curious of the poem itself

to pull at that thread of finding out more.

You picked a very particular part

that kind of made you want to pull at that thread,

and see where did these few lines come from?

Because I read it as a curiosity of a scientist,

those lines alone.

And also, as a desperate human being,

searching, like offering himself for an understanding,

or connection with another human being.

And then, because I wasn’t sure if it’s a love poem or not,

or if it’s desperation, or if it’s curiosity,

whatever it is, and then you see the love poem.

I mean, I don’t know, that’s gonna stick with me

for a while, your dark, rich life.

And then a few lines in here are just,

I mean, those are, I’m gonna just use them

as pickup lines at a bar.

I offer you the memory of a yellow rose

seen at sunset years before you were born.

No, that’s a pickup line I’ve never heard,

if I’ve ever heard one, anyway, sorry.

But this is universal.

You see it in so many forms of art.

Like, we’re in Texas now,

you see this in country and western songs.

It’s often a list of things.

Like, here’s how I describe myself.

There’s this, and there’s that,

and there’s the other thing, and here you are.

These things matter to me,

and I hope they matter to you, too.

It’s a pretty universal form,

but he did it in this very artful

and very vulnerable way.

It was both beautiful, and you could feel the hurt

coming from him, too, and that was important.

The dark stuff, too.

I offer you my ancestors, my dead men,

the ghosts that living men have honored in bronze,

and talking about two bullets through his lungs,

bearded and dead, wrapped by his soldiers

in the hide of a cow,

a mother’s grandfather just 24,

heading a charge of 300 men in Peru,

now ghosts on vanished horses.

So all of it, the whole history of it.

Since it is a love poem, what do you think about love?

Carl, what’s the role of love in the human condition?

We’ll talk about the dark stuff.

But maybe love is the dark stuff, too.

I mean, it’s the most powerful connection we can form,

and that’s what makes it so important to us.

It’s the strongest and most stable connection

that we can form with another person,

and that matters immensely.

It matters for the human family to have evolved

to be something that could survive against the odds

that we’ve faced over the years.

That unreasonable bond that becomes reasonable

by virtue of its own existence.

And of course, that joy, the wild, raw joy of love,

is not a bad thing, either.

So you put these together,

the strongest bridge we can form,

and the reward and the joy that it brings.

That’s what love is to me,

and from my perspective, this is something that,

it can be hard to capture fairly,

because you wanna talk about the positive

and the negative sides at once.

They need to be wrapped up together

for a full, honest description of what it is,

and that’s hard to do in a compact form.

And so you have to take time to talk about love.

You have to take time to do it justice.

It takes a book, or at least a poem.

Or several thousands of them.

I don’t know, could you pull up,

there’s a video I saw, yeah, like right here.

So can you pause for a second?

So there’s March of the Penguins.

So you always see penguins huddling together against,

I mean, sorry if I say just metaphors and everything,

but them huddling together against the harshness

of the conditions around them.

That’s very kind of, that’s like a metaphor for life,

like finding this connection.

That’s kind of what love is.

It’s like it allows you to forget whatever the absurdity,

whatever the suffering of life is,

together you get to like huddle for warmth.

And that’s why I love sort of just the honesty

and the intensity of the way penguins,

just in the middle of like the cold do this.

And then this video I saw, a lonely,

this is misinformation.

So the name of the video is Lonely Deranged Penguin.

I don’t know if he’s deranged.

So if you play it.

So he left his pack and there’s a nice like voiceover

and you don’t need to play it,

but he for some reason left the pack

and journeyed out into the mountains.

And so the narrator says that he’s deranged,

he’s lost his mind.

Now I’d like to project the idea that he’s actually,

there’s so many stories you could think of.

He’s returning to his homeland.

He’s an outsider thinking,

journeying out into the unknown,

thinking he may be able to discover

something greater than the tribe.

He might be looking for a lost love.

Why is he deranged immediately?

Why has he lost his mind?

Anyway, but this, people should look up this video

because to me, I might be the only one

who romanticizes this, but it’s such a nice kind of,

it’s both a picture of perhaps a mental disorder,

which is what the video kind of describes.

And it may be some deeper explanation

that’s not, that has to do with the motivation of a mind.

But yeah, I don’t know if you have a deeper analysis

on this penguin.

Well, I, like you as a psychiatrist,

I would want to sit down with a penguin and go through,

I want to see the notes from his prior therapist.

But this actually is relevant,

not knowing what was that penguin’s motivation.

We have very clear situations

where there are both within an individual,

we go through periods of time when we stay in one place

and we reap the benefits from what we’ve built.

And then we go through periods of foraging, of wandering.

Even if there may be resources where we are,

we have periods of time in our lives

where we wander, where we go in an exploratory mode.

And different people express that trait in different ways.

This is not a human specific trait.

If you go down to the tiny little nematode worm,

C. elegans with 302 nervous system cells,

they go through these phases of foraging and rest

and different individuals have different propensity

to forage or to rest and stay in one place.

At the level of the species, that’s really good

that there’s that diversity in their willingness to forage.

Some stay where they are,

the species is somewhat on a firm footing then,

but some carry a burden, a risk for themselves,

but it’s good for the species that they’re explorers

and they will venture out.

The migration patterns that different species blunder into

and that turned out to be really good,

they weren’t logically derived.

They most certainly started

from something like this, an exploration.

And humans do this too, you think?

And we do it too.

In fact, it’s something we do extremely well.

Let’s talk about psychiatry a little bit.

So in my book, you’re a rockstar.

First of all, for people who don’t know,

aside from sort of the neurological view of the brain

and neuroscience view of the brain,

you’re also one of the great psychiatrists of our time.

I’ve always, not always, but when I was younger,

I dreamed about being a psychiatrist.

So it’s like getting to meet your heroes

and also getting to meet the people who,

the best at the top of the world

at the thing you’ve failed to pursue.

So I’m getting a free therapy session on top of that.

Okay, so what big picture, what is the practice,

the goal, the hope of modern psychiatry?

If you could try to describe the discipline

as you see it, maybe historically

throughout the 20th century,

in contrasting to what it is today.

Or maybe if you want to describe

to what you hope psychiatry becomes

or longs to become in the 21st century.

It’s been an interesting journey.

Psychiatry started out pretty firmly grounded

in neurology and pathology.

Some of the initial founders effectively of the field

were very well grounded in microscopy, looking at cells,

working with patients, particularly on the neurological side,

and this certainly included Freud

and some of his contemporaries.

But they rapidly discovered that what they could work with

at the level of cells and microscopy

was so far from the realm of what they could get

from a human being and what they were getting

from the human being was so much more interesting

and was so mysterious and so unknown

that many of them just said,

we’re gonna inhabit this domain

and we’re gonna work with the people with their words

and understand what we can based on verbal communication,

because that was the only tool that people really had.

And that was a very important step for the field.

I would say one of the interesting things

that came from the early decades of psychiatry

really was this distinction

between the conscious and the unconscious mind

and paying particular attention to the unconscious mind

as something that was worthy of consideration

that might be important in explaining people’s actions

and that perhaps even insight into that was valuable

in its own right.

And out of that, psychoanalysis became a practice

that was not always focused on cures or treatments,

but was more focused on insight.

What does it mean?

How can we help people understand

why they’re feeling something or thinking something

or dreaming something?

And that insight separate even from treatment

was an interesting thing.

As long as one was honest about that

and said we’re going for understanding,

we’re going for insight.

Maybe it’s useful to just pause on that.

If we look at the father of psychoanalysis, Zygmunt Freud,

what do you make of the ideas that he had?

So you mentioned taking the unconscious,

the subconscious seriously.

That’s like step one,

like that there could be worlds

we do not have direct access for

and we probe at them through conversation

or is that too simplistic

to call psychoanalysis conversation?

That’s not too simplistic, but that’s right.

And I think that was valuable.

Where Freud ended up breaking

from some of his contemporaries,

he was very focused on this unconscious

as being so tightly linked to libido.

And really from his perspective,

you couldn’t really separate the operation

of the unconscious mind from these aspects

of the libidinous aspects.

And that was one reason.

What’s a libidinous aspect?

You know, sexually related drives.

Carl Jung, who was his contemporary,

that’s one factor that led to them separating

was Carl Jung felt there was a lot more

to the unconscious than this libidinous aspect of it.

And he saw it as a much more complete

alternate representation of the conscious self,

one that maybe reflected a whole range

of different motivations and desires.

And to properly treat it one had to consider all of them

rather than the ones that Freud was focused on.

Carl Jung, shut your mouth.

Thank you.

Thank you for the high level of images

that Sergei’s pulling up.

For people who are just listening,

he pulled up a quote from Sigmund Freud’s meme,

your mom quote Freud.

So the shadow, the Carl Jung shadow encompasses everything,

not just the desire to have sex with your mother

or sex period.

That’s right, that’s right.

If you look at those two folks en masse,

I mean, there’s a kind of,

it’s almost like a technique for philosophical exploration

of human mind, human motivations.

So it’s not even like necessarily,

it’s also doubles as a methodology for helping people,

but it’s almost like a,

it’s a kind of philosophical method.

Right, this is the fascinating thing about psychoanalysis.

And even though it’s, I would say,

mostly not considered a treatment today,

it persists for a couple of reasons.

One is it’s thought that it gives people some insight.

But second, there’s been a huge influence on literature,

on philosophy, on art.

And the opening up of discussion

about what was below our conscious mind

was so fertile in the implications

that it sort of reverberated and still does

throughout all these different realms of human endeavor

from different artistic experiences that people have

that can be colored by this concept of the unconscious.

Now, the other thing that was interesting

is this distinction,

you know, what are the parts of the unconscious?

And so there were these id and ego

and superego subdivisions that,

you know, that Freud, for example, would talk about them.

And the id was the primary, the primal drives

that an infant would have or that a very young child

just warmth and feeding and then later

the sexual or libidinous aspects.

And for Freud, the later happened very quickly.

That’s the controversial thing about him, I think.

I guess he thought like even children had sexual desires,

that they’re like dealing with, contending with.

So it’s the full thing.

Hungry, wanting to eat, wanting to poop,

wanting to have sex.

Yeah, and he was extremely focused on that aspect.

But then there was the superego,

which brought on these later sort of moralistic

sort of codes of conduct.

And that, of course, was very often in tension,

but all this could play out subconsciously.

And then the ego, this third aspect was mediating,

and Freud’s conception mediated this tension

between the different parts.

Now, I think that’s interesting.

I will say that in some ways, it’s maybe unnecessary

from the perspective of modern neuroscience

to divide things up that way from the moralistic drives

and the primal gratification drives.

In some ways, they’re all drives,

and maybe they’re even all primal drives.

The moralistic drives, they’re taught,

and they’re taught in ways that ultimately relate

back to survival, and you could even say,

selfish aspects of health and life

for the self and family.

And so this is, I think it’s maybe an artificial distinction.

The concept of the unconscious is very valuable

and very interesting, but these categorizations

of id and superego may not map onto neurobiology

in any particular way.

If there’s a town hall of competing drives and desires,

and they interrelate to each other,

they involve different aspects of the brain

and the history of the person,

and actions and choices come out of the result

of that overall shouting in the town hall.

So in some sense, Carl Jung was a step into the direction

of liberating yourself from such harsh categorizations.

Do you think, I mean, you have Daniel Kahneman

with System One and System Two.

There’s just these very compelling categorizations

of the human mind that seem to be sticky

in the superego, no, in how we talk about these ideas

and so on.

Do you think those are helpful or do they get in the way?

Is it some kind of balance in terms of deeper understanding

of how the mind actually works?

You know, it’s from modern neuroscience,

whenever we seem to get closer to addressing a question

like this at the level of cells,

it seems to get farther away.

And I’ll give you an example of what I mean by that.

So one thing I’m doing in my laboratory

and many people are doing is we are listening in

on the activity of cells, neurons in the brain

of mice or rats or fish or monkeys.

Individual cells.

Individual cells, exactly.

Of which there are, in our brain, many billions.

And when we do and we try to predict

what action will be taken by an animal

to address this question, where does the choice arise?

Where does the impetus to make a particular selection

of one action versus another action,

where does that start in the brain?

If you’re recording, listening in on the activity of cells

all across the brain, where’s the earliest spot

you can pick up a choice being made?

Well?

That’s so awesome.

Yeah, at one level, you might think,

how excited would Jung have been to see this

or Freud or the early psychoanalyst

to see where this starts.

But it’s not so simple because an emerging theme

in very recent neuroscience,

literally over the last few years,

is that things sort of all start together

all across the brain.

And so you can be recording from the cortex,

this rim of cells at the surface of the brain,

or you can be recording deeper

in a structure called the striatum,

which is a little older.

It’s more tightly linked to action.

And then structures called the thalamus,

other parts of the brain.

And if you record from these,

these all sort of represent the action and the choice

more or less all at about the same time, very close.

And so you can’t point to a particular spot and say,

here’s where the choice or the action originates.

It’s a group.

Finding the free will neuron.

It’s relevant to that question.

Nobody is close to being able to point to such a thing.

Well, close is a relative term.

And nobody, what I tweet today,

all generalizations are wrong.

So including this one.

Let’s actually talk about that.

So the study of individual cells.

If you could linger on your sense

that as you get closer to that understanding,

it feels like you’re getting farther away.

Why is that?

Because that often is the feeling

until you’re actually there.

So like, you know, see that’s when I’m running

and I know there’s only a mile left,

it just feels like that mile

is just getting longer and longer,

but eventually you finish.

So maybe we’re getting close

to cracking open these beginnings of a sense,

like we’ll talk about consciousness

or these very difficult, big questions

about the human mind.

Where do they start?

You’re right to say we shouldn’t generalize

or make absolutist statements,

but I would say right now,

the reason things are looking even harder to crack

than we had initially thought,

we now have the data streams

that we’ve wanted for so long

in terms of activity patterns all across the brain

at the level of cells.

We can literally see what the cells are doing.

Immense data sets.

You know, we get, these are time series

of one individual cell with sub second resolution

and you can collect this from enormous numbers of cells

across the brain.

So very rich data sets that we’ve wanted for a long time

and yet having these has not led

to an understanding of truly where actions

initiate in terms of regions or locations.

And let’s get a few questions on that.

Is the answer, high level question by your intuition,

is the answer within the data?

Or do we need different kind of data?

So we should also say that when you collect data

about the brain, there’s like the richness

of information you’re collecting,

but there’s also human doing stuff.

And information, so static information about the human

and dynamic information about the human

and you can get them to do different stuff

and you can select different humans

and that’s part of the collection of data aspect.

So like when you’re collecting data about the brain,

there’s some truths that you can,

you know, in machine learning is like annotations,

like supervised learning.

There’s some true things you can hold on to

before you look at the full rich mess complexity

of the human mind.

So given the data you’ve looked at,

do you think the answer for the origin of free will

in the human mind can be found?

Well, one amazing thing is that nobody’s found it,

but we have these rich data sets

and then there’s a conundrum which is,

is it in the data and we just don’t know

how to look at it.

Maybe we don’t know the right scale,

the right projection to make of the data,

the right way to interpret it.

And here’s where causal testing becomes very valuable

because then instead of just passively observing,

well, here are the activity patterns

and then here’s the choice made by the animal.

As we’ve gotten more powerful at reaching in

and causing things to happen in the brain,

turning up or down the activity

of certain types of cells or defined populations of cells

and seeing how that affects actions,

these causal perturbations have turned out

to be very valuable.

We’re just now getting to the point

where we can apply these in very wide swaths of the brain

at cellular resolution and so we’re gonna be able,

hopefully to make some headway on this question

with causality and that’s the one thing

that optogenetics provides us this way of using light

that we develop to control cells.

This is an untapped, relatively untapped

at this broad brain wide scale

and hopefully we can get there in the near future.

But I would say that the answer may be in the data

but we don’t know how to find it.

Well, there’s this interactive element

like where you can cause stuff that’s really powerful

because you get to, I mean,

as opposed to collecting data passively,

you get your collecting data actively.

So can you maybe describe one of the many things

you’re known for, one of the big things

is called optogenetics, what is it?

Optogenetics is a way of causing things to happen.

It’s a way of determining what actually matters

in terms of the activity of the brain

for the amazing things it does,

sensation, cognition, action.

And what it does is it provides activity.

It’s a way of playing in, if you will,

activity patterns into precisely defined cells.

And the way we do it is pretty cool, I think.

It’s, you know, right away there’s a problem

if you think about how do we do this?

How could we play in well defined activity patterns

and provide a stream of activity into this cell

and that cell and that cell but not these other cells?

So just for context, we’re talking about the brains

of mice, monkeys, humans,

and then the goal is to try to control accurately

the behavior of a single neuron

and then to be able to monitor single collection

of single neurons to then say, well,

to draw some deeper insight about the origins,

first of all, the function of different parts of the brain,

different neurons, different kinds of neurons,

but also the origins of the big things,

the flap of the butterfly wing that leads

to an actual behavioral thing.

Yeah, so if you could, exactly, so if you could turn on

or off the brain or parts of the brain or cell types

or individual cells at the natural rate and rhythm

and timing of normal brain activity,

that would be immensely valuable

because you could determine what actually mattered,

what could cause complex things to happen

and what could prevent complex things from happening

in a specific way.

But right away, you’ve got a problem if you wanna do this

and scientists, neuroscientists have wanted to do this

for a long time.

Francis Crick of Double Helix of DNA fame,

he wrote a famous paper in 1999.

He got interested in neuroscience later in life

and he said, what we need in neuroscience is a way

that we could turn on or off the activity

of individual types of neurons in a behaving animal.

And he even said the ideal signal would be light

because it would be fast, it could penetrate

through the brain to some extent.

And he had no idea how to do it.

He said this would probably be very farfetched,

but it would be a good thing.

And so that’s what you’re actually saying,

like if you wanna do this kind of thing

and you imagine like, how do I get inside the brain?

It’s pretty difficult.

It’s pretty difficult and then even once you get in,

it’s hard because all brain cells are electrical,

all neurons are electrically activated.

And so if you wanted to use electricity

as what you were putting in,

you won’t have any specificity at all.

If you have an electrode, a wire,

and you put it in the brain and you send current through it,

all the cells near the electrode will be stimulated.

That’s like trying to control fish

by spraying them with water.

Yeah, right, because there’s already a lot of electricity

going around anyway and you’re adding more,

but there’s no specificity

even among the different kinds of cells either

because all around the wire that you’ve put in,

there are gonna be so many different cells

doing totally different things,

many of them in opposition to each other.

We know that’s one way the brain is set up.

There are parts of the brain

where neurons side by side

are doing completely different things

and maybe even antagonistic to each other.

So what do you do?

How do you play in activity with any kind of specificity?

Well, what you do is use,

what we found is what you can do is

make some cells responsive to light.

Now, normally no cells deep in the brain

really respond to light.

They’re not built for that.

There’s no reason for them to respond to light in there,

which is a great situation to start with

because any light sensitivity you can provide to some cells

will be a huge signal above the noise.

And so that’s what we do with optogenetics.

We take genes, bits of DNA from microbes,

single celled organisms,

and these single celled organisms like algae,

they make little proteins

that sit in the surface of their cells

that receive light, capture a photon of light

and open up a little hole in the membrane of the cell

and let charged particles, ions like sodium and potassium

flow across the membrane of the cell.

And that, these algae and bacteria,

they do this for their own reasons

because that helps them move,

it helps them make and use energy.

But that’s a beautiful thing for neuroscience

because movement of ions,

charged particles across the membrane of the cell

is exactly the kind of electricity that neurons work with.

So if we can take this bit of DNA

that encodes this beautiful protein

that turns light into electricity from algae,

and if we can put it into some neurons,

but not other neurons, which we can do using genetic tricks,

then you’ve got a situation,

then you can shine on the light

and only the cells that have the gene

and that are expressing the gene

will be the initial direct cells

that are activated by the light.

And so that’s the essence of optogenetics

is the ability to do that.

We get that initial specificity

that you could never get with an electrode.

So let me say that this is,

we recently got the Alaska Prize for this.

It’s a brilliant idea.

So I talked to Andrew Huberman,

who’s a friend of yours, friend of mine,

so not to jinx things,

but he believes that you deserve the Nobel Prize for this.

So, I do too, but what, my votes.

Anyway, the thing is, it doesn’t matter.

Prizes will be all forgotten, all of us will be forgotten.

When the cool idea is a cool idea,

that’s a really powerful idea.

It’s actually, the origins of it

you might be interested in are even, are very deep.

There was a botanist in St. Petersburg

named Andre Fomensen.

In 1866, he published a paper

on the single celled green algae.

And he was the botanist who first noticed

that they moved in response to light.

These are tiny single celled algae that have flagella,

so they swim through the water.

And he noticed this, he was a botanist,

and he published this.

It was a paper, you know, he wrote in German,

but he published it in a French journal,

and he was doing it from St. Petersburg,

so it was a very international effort.

But you have to go back to 1866,

and that, I like to highlight how far back

that discovery goes is back to Andre Fomensen.

And this is a, it highlights the value

of just pure basic science discovery.

That always originates somewhere

in the Eastern European block.

But I don’t think he expected the splicing

of genetic material from the algae into the human brain.

And one of the cool things we’ve been able to do now

with modern methods is to really study these proteins.

And so we’ve discovered some of these proteins,

other groups have as well.

We’ve dived deep into their structure,

just like the double helix structure of DNA

was uncovered with X ray crystallography.

We used the same method in X ray crystallography

to see how these beautiful little proteins work.

We reengineered them for all kinds of function.

We can make them, instead of responding to blue light,

we can make them respond to red light.

We can speed them up, slow them down.

We can make them, with genetic engineering,

we can make them have different ions flow through them.

And so it’s this convergence, as you said,

like the botanist in 1866 couldn’t have predicted

what we could do with this.

And the fact that we’ve been able to discover

how these beautiful proteins work

and then apply them to neuroscience

is really a thrilling story.

Is it possible to achieve scale, do you think, with this?

Meaning, like what is the progress of the next 50 years,

100 years looks like in terms of the precision

and the scale of control of using light?

It’s going so fast it’s hard to predict.

I’ll give you a sense of it though.

First paper we published in 2005,

that was just encultured neurons.

By 2007, so that was in a dish.

By 2007, we had it working in behaving mice.

By 2009, we had it pretty general.

So we had methods to really make it a versatile method.

It could be applied to essentially any cell.

By 2012, we could get to single cell resolution.

We used light guidance strategies

to target individual cells in the brain of a living mouse.

By 2019, we were able to control up to 20 to 50

individually specified single cells

in the brain of a mouse in ways that specifically changed

its behavior, that could bias its decisions one

way or the other.

In fact, we could take a mouse and without any visual stimulus

at all, we could make it act as if it

had seen a particular visual stimulus

by playing in, using the single cell resolution optogenetics,

a specific pattern of activity into 20 or 25

individually specified cells.

That’s 2019 to your question of scale.

Now in 2022, we’re controlling hundreds

of individually specified single cells

over all of visual cortex of a mouse, all the part

of the brain that is the initial direct target

of the incoming information from the retina.

Are you constrained to specific types of cells currently?

Like you mentioned, long range is easier.

Is there constraints on which cells?

Now there really isn’t.

Now that we have this individual cell guidance,

we can target any individual kind of cell very reliably.

And so now to your question of scale, how far can we go?

Well, things are moving quickly.

It’s hard to say.

We can access individual cells across the entire brain now.

If you look 10, 20 years in the future,

I think we’ll surprise ourselves.

But the fact that we’re already able to cause

specific perceptions to happen and specific actions

means we’re essentially where we want to be.

And now it’s a matter of just more experiments,

more discoveries.

But the basic principles are clear now.

The basic capability is there.

Is there a pathway to doing the same for humans?

Optogenetics is primarily, it’s a discovery tool

that really is well suited for use in mice and rats

and monkeys because it involves putting in a gene

and also delivering light.

And those are two things that you can do in human beings,

but you’d want to do in a very careful way.

Now that said, there is actually just less than a year ago,

my friend Botan Droska in Switzerland,

he did the first human optogenetics therapy.

And he published this in the journal Nature Medicine.

So about 10, 12 years ago, he and I

published a paper together where we gave him

one of our optogenetic tools, one

of these light activated regulators of ion flow.

These are called microbial opsins, by the way, opsins.

And he put one of those into an extracted retina

from a human being who had died.

So it was a cadaveric retina.

And he was able to show that optical control in this paper

was able to turn on or off individual cells

in the human retina.

So that was a while back.

He spent about 10 years of going through all the regulatory

hoops and hurdles and going through primate studies.

And finally, he was able to take a human being

with a retinal degeneration syndrome, so someone

who was blind in both eyes.

And he gave one of these opsins into one eye of this human

being who was blind and with the goal

of conferring light sensitivity onto this retina that

was not able to see light.

And he was able to make this person see through that eye.

So he took a blind person.

And the blind person could see now,

could reach for objects selectively on a table.

And he published this in Nature Medicine.

And it was, you know, that’s an amazing thing.

Do you know the title of the paper?

What’s his name again?

Rosca, R O S K A.

And you look up the Nature paper.

Nature Medicine.

So that’s sort of proof of principle.

Now, the retina is very accessible.

It’s near the surface.

You can use natural light, or you

can use brighter natural light.

I’m myself, I see optogenetics as a discovery tool.

It’s a way to figure out the principles by which the brain

works and how it operates.

Partial recovery of visual function

in a blind patient after optogenetic therapy.

So he went through the full process of doing primates

and then going, wow, that’s dedication

and that’s really exciting to see.

As beautiful as that is, and I’m glad he did all that work,

there are so many other ways that optogenetics

could help with therapies.

Once you know the principles, then any kind of therapy

can become more powerful.

Once you know the causal cells in a symptom,

like in lack of motivation or inability

to enjoy things or altered sleep or altered energy,

once you know the cells that are causal,

then you can make medications that address those cells.

You could address brain stimulation treatments that

might address those cells.

Also, diagnosis, very effective systematic way of diagnosing,

or at least providing you rich data

to some of these deep questions about schizophrenia,

about bipolar, all of those kinds of things.

The tools are low resolution currently

for determining the degree to which you have a thing

and whether you have a thing at all.

Yeah, exactly.

And so the hope is that’s a great example

of how you can cure or you can provide

some relief for a symptom of a person who has

a serious degenerative disease.

But the principles are what we’re after,

and that’s why I spend, even though I’m a psychiatrist,

even though I still see patients, I’m not myself

trying to drive any clinical trials in the lab.

I’m trying to discover, and then any kind of therapy

could result from that.

What do you think about my friend,

Elon Musk, and his efforts with Neuralink?

So this is another, there’s a lot of things to say here,

because there’s a lot of ideas under the umbrella

of Neuralink, but one of them is to use electrical signals

to stimulate, and then you also record,

you collect electrical signals from the brain

at a higher and higher resolution,

and you go implant surgically the methods

by which you do the stimulation and the data collection.

So it’s possible for the ideas of optogenetics

to play well with this, and we can even zoom out

outside of just Neuralink, and just the whole idea

of brain computer interfaces.

What are your thoughts?

Well, I think the engineering that they’ve done

is actually pretty cool.

So I like the.

Robots.

Yeah, from the design perspective,

and it was a design approach that wasn’t being taken

in academia, and it’s great that they did it,

and I think it’s pretty cool.

So I’ll say that.

Also, there are many ways that you can record

from many thousands of neurons, and that’s not the only way.

It’s a very interesting way.

We and others are using brain penetrating electrodes

that actually get quite deep.

This whole structure of the brain is very interesting.

There’s the surface cortex, where it’s the most recently

emergent part of the brain in evolution.

Mammals have it.

Reptiles have something a little bit like it,

but it’s not really the full thing.

This is a very recent thing.

That’s what we can access with some of these,

like the Neuralink approach,

and with some of these short electrodes.

This part of the brain, the cortex,

is only a few millimeters thick.

There’s so much that’s deep, though, that’s so important.

There’s the striatum, there’s the thalamus.

There are the parts of the brain that drive motivation,

that drive hunger and thirst and social interaction

and parenting and flight and fear and anxiety.

All these things are, there’s so much that’s deep

that these surface approaches are not getting to.

And so we and others are using these very long electrodes

that help us get deep, and we can still record

for many cells, many thousands of cells.

We can have multiple of these at once in the same animal.

And so there’s a diversity of methods to get to this goal.

I think it’s great that people coming from

outside academia will bring ideas

that weren’t being worked on, at least approaches.

They may turn out to be synergistic.

These things do work very well with optogenetics

because all these electrical recording methods,

that’s one channel of information flow.

Light delivery is a separate, more or less independent.

There can be some artifacts that happen,

but if you’re careful,

it’s another independent pathway of information flow.

And we’ve done really fun experiments in mice

where we play in patterns of activity with light,

and we record activity from across the brain of a mouse

electrically, and so using optical and electrical together

is extremely powerful.

So like optoelectric brain computer interfaces.

Which, by the way, there’s efforts on the computing side

to build optoelectric servers,

so like where you have both electricity.

So because optics is really interesting,

light is a very interesting method of communication

that’s, like you said, orthogonal in many ways.

It doesn’t have some of the constraints of bandwidth

that electricity does going through wires,

but you’re able to,

but less ability to control precisely at scale.

So like there’s challenges and there’s benefits,

and having those two interplays

really, really, really fascinating,

especially when obviously on the other side

of your signal is a biological mesh, mush, mushy mesh.

Well, the mushy mesh is kind of interesting

because there are problems with light.

Light scatters in the brain,

so the photons don’t just go linearly through.

Whenever they hit an interface between fat and water,

lipid and water, they bounce off in different directions.

And so you can come in with all the resolution you want.

You could play in an incredibly detailed,

high resolution pattern of light,

but the photons start scattering quite quickly,

and by the time you’ve gone a couple of millimeters deep,

you’ve lost almost all that fine spatial information.

So, but we’ve developed workarounds.

The longer wavelength light you use,

if you get into the infrared, there’s less scattering.

You can use two photon methods or three photon methods

where the photons have to arrive all together

at the same time.

You can put in fiber optics.

We developed these fiber optic methods in 2007

where you can access these deep structures

with fiber optic methods,

and you can put many of these fiber optics

at the same time in an animal.

We’ve used holographic methods, 3D holograms,

to play in hundreds of individual cell size spots of light,

and we can change those quickly.

So there are a lot of tricks,

a lot of interesting optics engineering

that has come together with neuroscience

in a pretty exciting way.

Well, that is engineering, too.

It was just super, super, super exciting.

I should mention, because I remember I mentioned Elon.

I recently got, for the first time ever, got COVID.

Well, how did I go so long without,

finally, so I’m all vaccinated and everything like that.

And so I got, because I think he mentioned it publicly

so I can mention it,

but I won’t mention anybody else involved.

But hanging out, we all got, Elon got COVID.

And the interesting thing about,

maybe you can comment about this.

So I was only sick for like a half a day.

I got a fever of like 104.

I just went up and then crashed.

And then I was, now maybe I’m just seeing

the silver lining of everything,

but afterwards, I have like a greater clarity

about the world.

You just think it’s greater clarity.

Maybe, maybe I just, it was so,

maybe so intensely the mind fog kind of thing

for such a short amount of time.

But the people who were involved were also reporting this.

It’s kind of interesting.

It’s like, because I do know like the immune system

is involved with the brain in very interesting ways.

So like the human mind also incorporates all these other,

it’s not just the, it’s not just the nervous system.

And I just wonder, because everyone always says,

no, not like, everyone always says like COVID

does all these bad things or whatever the disease is

or whatever the virus.

But I wonder like, I hate to be a Steven Pinker on this,

but like, I wonder what the benefits of certain disease are

if you’re able to recover.

Like what, is there some like, again,

don’t want to romanticize it,

but if your system goes to some kind of hardship

and you come out on the other end,

I wonder sometimes if there’s a greater,

maybe killed off a bunch of neurons

that didn’t need anyway,

and they were actually getting in the way.

There were the hater neurons.

I don’t know.

Well, that was your inner critic that I was talking about.

Exactly.

You killed off your critic.

Well, you know, there are mechanisms for what you,

the potential mechanisms for what you’re talking about.

There are, there’s actually been a fair bit of research

on post COVID neurological function.

Actually, my wife, Michelle Monjay, who’s at Stanford,

she’s done a lot of this work.

Akiko Iwasaki at Yale has done a lot of this.

But what they found is that there’s a loss of myelin.

This is the coating of those long range projections

that go from one part of the brain to another.

Myelin is this sort of insulator

that coats these long range projections

and makes the impulses go faster and more reliably.

And there’s altered function of the myelin producing cells

and altered myelin in the case of COVID.

They’ve looked in both mouse and human brains.

And, but of course it could be very idiosyncratic.

Many people have cognitive problems post COVID.

You’re definitely aware of that.

So many people report this persistent brain fog

and the ability to function.

But it depends on where the inflammation was.

Maybe the people who have dysfunction post COVID,

they had a global effect.

Maybe you lost some of these projections

that were restraining you in some way.

And these plausibly exist.

And it’s known that there are cell populations

in the prefrontal cortex that actively restrain

deeper structures from expressing what they do.

And it’s theoretically possible that you had a lucky.

Somebody has to get lucky, right?

Somebody has to get lucky, yeah.

Why not me?

All right, if we can actually go back to this idea

of trying through optogenetics

to find origins of when the wave first starts.

Origins of a decision.

Origin of idea.

Origin of maybe consciousness

or the subjective experience.

So origin of things in the mind.

So one thing, Carl Jung, is there a God neuron?

Is there a belief neuron?

Is there, so through this methodology of optogenetics,

can you start getting to where a belief begins

or an idea begins?

And especially looking at the strongest of our beliefs.

Maybe beliefs of love and hate,

but religious belief into something really grand,

on the grandest of scale.

Yeah, neuroscience and neurology point us a little bit.

We don’t have an answer to that, but for example.

A lot of these questions I’m gonna ask you,

there’s no good answer, but you’re providing the tools

that give us hope to find the answer one day.

Yeah, and we have early clues.

So for example, when patients with epilepsy

have experiences of religiosity as part of their seizure

or the aura before their seizure,

very often those are in the temporal lobe,

in these parts of the brain that are at the side.

And so that’s an initial clue.

There are also parts of the brain that are involved

in the definition of the self

and defining the borders or boundaries of the self.

And we know this, this is some experiments that we did

in my lab, there’s a part of the brain

where if there’s a rhythm of a particular type,

you can cause a separation of the sense of self

from the sense of the body.

What’s normally bound up and unitary,

we normally think of ourself and our body

as pretty tightly bound up together,

those can be separated, it turns out.

We can’t take that for granted.

And there are certain conditions,

certain patterns of activity in one part of the brain

called the retro splenial cortex,

where you can actually separate those two out.

And so if you think about these very big questions,

you know, what is, where are the origins of religiosity?

Where, how do we define the boundaries of who we are

relative to others and to the world?

How do we link ourself to our body

and how can that become separated?

These are actually, believe it or not,

now accessible and rigorously and quantitatively so.

We did an experiment with optogenetics

where we provided this abnormal rhythm

to this particular part of the mouse brain

and we saw this separation of detection of a stimulus

and caring about it.

So that’s like stimulating something about the mouse brain

that affects these neurons that give the conception of self.

So you’re able to dissociate the experience

from the impact of the experience onto you.

That’s right, exactly right.

So like these are the goals of meditation.

These are the goals whenever I get drunk,

pretty much effective.

I mean, that’s not a scientific statement,

just an experiential anecdotal one.

Also psychedelics seek to this,

to attain this kind of state.

That’s so interesting.

Well, you mentioned psychedelics, you know,

DMT and 5MeO DMT, these create this religious experience,

this connection, people describe them

as a strong connection to God.

That in theory, these are accessible with modern methods.

Now that we have these rich recording methods,

we can explore what are the precise millisecond resolution,

cellular resolution, brain wide manifestations

of these altered states.

So like you could look at an altered state like on DMT,

record it across many people,

and then from there see where do these experiences

originate in the brain in terms of single neurons,

and then how do they propagate

and interact with everything else?

And if there’s some kind of common signal,

like how do you narrow down the set of neurons

that are responsible for particular experience

or for a particular behavioral effect?

Yeah, here’s where optogenetics is so useful

because anytime you give an agent like ketamine or PCP,

which we used for our dissociation experiments

that I was mentioning,

or you have a psychedelic LSD or DMT

for this altered perceptual state,

if you give either of those,

these change everything across the brain, okay?

So just the fact that you maybe give them to a mouse,

let’s say, or eventually to a human,

you won’t know yet which cells to home in on

as the causal players in all this

just by recording the activity.

But then what we found is that optogenetics

providing a causal pattern of activity

guided by what you see can let you test hypotheses.

And we saw this rhythm with ketamine and PCP

for dissociation, and then we said, okay,

let’s test what’s causal.

We came in and provided that rhythm.

We tried a few different things,

but only one of the causal tests we tried

actually caused the behavioral dissociation.

And so that’s how we home in on what actually matters.

And is it repeatable once you see the causality?

So like that’s one definition of causality

is like you try and it repeats across different mice

and all that kind of stuff.

Exactly.

And so you could do that for DMT.

You could do that for the really fascinating

mind expanding, thank you, thank you.

So the meme for people just listening,

this is again another disagreement

between Freud and Carl Jung.

Religion and spirituality.

This is the, I guess the ring scene from Lord of the Rings.

Religion and spirituality, Freud says,

cast it into the fire, destroy it.

Carl Jung says, no.

So for people who don’t know,

Sergei is the Slavic Lord of the Meme.

Thank you, I appreciate that.

So what we’re talking about,

so there is, I mean, I think a connection

between DMT and religious experiences

are some of these psychedelics.

Do you think it’s possible to

sort of stimulate religious experiences?

And so religious experiences are one of the most

deep kind of experiences.

And so here you could first understand

where they originate, how they propagate

through the brain, and then to stimulate them.

And so this is, and these can happen

in people who had no predisposition.

People who are as agnostic or atheistic as you’d like,

they can have these, they can feel connected

to God in these states.

Now, to be clear, I’m not advocating these.

We don’t know what’s safe in human beings,

but we definitely have not yet.

But we definitely can do these experiments in mice,

and that was already very productive

in understanding dissociation.

So we can already imagine making headway on these methods.

And then I had a, and this does map

onto the non psychedelic human experience.

I had a patient who was actually described

in the book Projections.

This was the patient that’s in the mania chapter,

the bipolar chapter.

Here was a guy who had never had a psychiatric illness

or symptom in his life.

He was a retirement age gentleman,

and nobody in his family either.

So no family history, no personal history

of any psychiatric illness, and he’d never been religious,

particularly before either.

Certainly no passionate type of religion.

But he, not through any psychedelic or drug,

he had a stressful experience,

actually a post 9 11 change in how he was thinking.

And he was pushed into a mania, a manic state,

revealing that he had bipolar,

never before known in this case, in this person.

And his mania, his elevated state in bipolar

included this profound religiosity,

which he had never had before.

And he was preaching in a elevated,

vigorous way to his family.

And so this state can be created in people

even late in life who had no predisposition for it

and even without a neurochemical.

So the causality of that is very interesting to explore.

How did the manic state unleash this religiosity?

But you see that in other realms of psychiatry too.

OCD can manifest as religiosity also.

You can take people who never really had

a religion, never played a powerful role in their life,

but then when their obsessive compulsive symptoms

become severe, they can manifest in this.

I think I’m in that group, so I’m a bit OCD.

We have, I think this is subreddits,

when there’s oddly satisfying things.

So there’s certain things that are really satisfying

to my OCD, in my mild OCD.

I think it’s pretty much a religious experience.

So I understand that if it’s not direct,

it’s at least rhymes.

So maybe can you speak to the,

Sergei’s probably desperately scrambling

to pull up oddly satisfying, thank you.

People can check it out themselves.

It is, as the subreddit promises, oddly satisfying.

Can we talk about bipolar and maybe depression?

Well, let’s talk about, I mean, I don’t know if there’s

a nice way to discuss the differences

in the full landscape of suffering that’s here,

but maybe what is depression?

And what are the types of depression?

What kind of depression have you seen and experienced

and researched and how can people overcome it?

How can humans overcome it and deal with it,

live with it and overcome it?

So this is my clinical specialty.

I see patients in my outpatient clinical work

with treatment resistant depression.

So very hard to treat severe illness

where medications haven’t been working.

I also see patients with autism spectrum disorders.

These are my two clinical focal areas,

but then I do emergency room work as well.

But the depression, why do I focus on that?

It’s so, one feels tantalizingly close to helping

these people who are suffering so deeply.

And that’s why I focused on it is these are people who,

there may not even be anything situational

that’s difficult or challenging in their life.

You can have people who seem to have everything

that you would want.

Every objective measure of their life is fine,

yet they can be just hit with this unstoppable hopelessness

and inability to see into the future,

a discounting of the value of their own action.

Anything they can imagine themselves doing seems worthless

or they are unable to enjoy things.

We call this anhedonia.

There’s no reward, no pleasure, not in food,

social interaction, movies, books,

anything that they would enjoy, positivity gone.

They can have a profound negative internal state,

psychic pain, and these things can seem,

and in the severe cases, are inescapable.

So what is going on?

Why is this state part of human existence?

It’s got a strong biological, genetic link, we know that.

It’s been linked to certain genes,

certain regions of the chromosomes, and twin studies.

There’s a clear genetic link.

It doesn’t explain everything, but it’s a big part of it.

Genetics are a strong contributor.

And although you can have depression

without anything terrible going on in your life,

the symptoms can be made worse by stressors, by trauma.

But at a very deep level,

there’s nothing we can measure in a person objectively,

so we don’t have, there’s not a known chemical,

not a known structure that’s different,

not a known brain activity pattern

that we can pick up with EEG.

A lot of people are exploring this,

but right now we have no objective measures.

All we do is talk to people and we elicit these symptoms.

We explore them, distinguish them from other possible causes,

and then what do we do?

Well, we have a lot of things that we can do.

Well, we have a range of treatments.

We have medications that can help people,

do help people, but not everybody.

And if they don’t work,

then we can go to brain stimulation methods.

We can do things even like electroconvulsive therapy,

which is very effective,

but it’s sort of the final thing we go to in the end.

And so we have treatments.

They work for some people.

They don’t do everything we’d like.

But here’s the problem is at a very deep level,

we don’t understand really what’s going on in the brain.

We don’t have a physical interpretation of the problem.

We have all these symptoms,

but we can’t yet point to a set of cells

or a set of circuits or an activity pattern

that is causing major depression,

this disease state per se in human beings.

Why do you think you can’t yet

from an optogenetics perspective?

Is it because there’s so many possible causes?

Is it so many things involved?

So I think the answer is there are many things involved

and all these different symptoms that I’ve mentioned,

those we can study and those we can fix,

the individual symptoms.

And we can do this in animals to be clear.

So in a mouse, for example,

we can instantaneously and precisely

turn up or down the motivation of an animal

to overcome a challenge.

We can turn up or down its ability

to be motivated by, or we think experience reward

from situations or actions.

We can increase its apparent energy level,

its drive to meet challenges.

We can turn up or down social interaction.

All these individual features of depression,

individual symptoms, we now can point to exact projections

and cells that are causal in mediating these.

But we don’t know is why all these different symptoms

show up together in major depression

and the human disease syndrome.

And that’s the mystery.

It’s sort of, in other fields of medicine,

someone with congestive heart failure

who comes into the clinic,

they have very different symptoms.

They have shortness of breath and they have swollen feet.

Couldn’t be two more different across the body

sets of symptoms.

Neither one obviously related to the heart,

but they’re both happening

because the heart is not working as a pump, okay?

And now, thankfully in cardiology,

we understand these disparate symptoms

that seem totally unrelated can be completely understood

because there’s an altered pump action of the heart.

That’s what we are hoping for in psychiatry

and in the study of depression or any disease.

These different symptoms,

the inability to enjoy things, the hopelessness.

What’s the unifying principle?

Yeah, unifying.

I mean, is there some truth to that Tolstoy quote

that all happy families are alike

and each unhappy family is unhappy in its own way?

So basically, I mean, this is the human condition.

And basically, the physicists long to find

a theory of everything, isn’t understanding depression

essentially require you to really have

the big theory of everything for the human mind?

I think we, it would certainly be nice to have that,

a theory of everything.

Don’t get me wrong.

I don’t think we need it.

The understatement of the century, it would be nice.

Well, it’s also a good question if it’s possible.

Yeah, yeah.

Well, that I have some thoughts on too.

But to this specific question,

I don’t think we need a theory of everything.

I think there will be unifying principles we can get to.

But even shy of that, we can treat symptoms

and that’s a big step.

And as you say, different unhappy families are different,

different unhappy people are different.

If we have somebody who comes to the clinic

and I see someone with a profound anhedonia

as one of their main symptoms,

inability to enjoy things,

and if I know based on optogenetics work and animal work

that a particular medication can treat anhedonia,

even if it doesn’t fix major depression in everybody,

if I treat that one symptom in that one person,

that’s a good thing.

And so we don’t need the theory of everything

and we don’t even need the unifying principle

to help people with insights that come from optogenetics.

How much does talking help for diagnosis

and for treatment, would you say, for depression?

It’s a big part of what we do.

Every good psychiatrist should be pretty adept

in these verbal communications and talk therapy

as part of what they do.

I give medications, I deliver brain stimulation treatments,

but a big, big part of everything I do with every patient

is talk therapy because it works so well together

with these other modalities.

Even alone, it can help people with moderate

or mild depression by itself.

People with severe depression,

people with other psychiatric illnesses that are severe,

you don’t wanna do talk therapy alone,

that’s not gonna do it.

But it still is crucial to do together with the others.

And it’s critical because it’s part of how

you reshape cognitions, complex activity patterns,

and you won’t get to that with a medication

or a brain stimulation treatment.

Do you have advice for people who suffer

from mild forms of depression or feel as they might,

both for those people, and do you have advice

for people who love the people who suffer

from depression and want to help?

Yeah, one of the incredibly frustrating things

about depression is the very nature of it makes it hard

for the people who suffer to get treatment

because they’re hopeless,

so they don’t think treatment will help.

They have low energy, so they’re not motivated

to participate in treatment in many cases.

Sometimes they’re actively suicidal.

That certainly doesn’t help.

They have all these things that seem to prevent treatment

from being effective.

So the loved ones, that’s where the loved ones

are so important, is helping them overcome these barriers

to treatment, the motivation, the safety, and the insight.

That’s critical, and particularly for the severe cases.

For the mild cases, where people still have some insight

and motivation and energy to get something done,

there are many things you can do.

Exercise is extremely important in mood maintenance.

Regulation of sleep and getting sufficient

and regular enough sleep is very important.

And talk therapy can be helpful in those mild

or moderate cases, just looking at cognitions,

looking at patterns of thought that people

may have fallen into, where they catastrophize,

where they spiral from small things into big things.

A little bit of talk therapy, 10, 12 sessions,

can help people identify those patterns they may have

in themselves that are taking occasional negative thoughts,

which everybody has, and magnifying those

into more persistent negative states.

If you work at this, and it’s kind of like homework,

this is what we call cognitive behavioral therapy.

It’s very structured, very organized.

You work hard.

It requires insight and motivation,

and you have to be motivated.

But if you are, then you can identify these triggers

that send you down particular pathways.

And work to intercept them.

And that is amazingly very effective

in mild to moderate cases.

So you basically have to train yourself

to see the world as a collection of triggers.

And you have to first understand, like collect the data,

like basically see every experience

as a thing that creates a follow on emotion, a feeling.

And like, I’ve learned this, you know, like on social media,

where like early on, you know, like all of us,

you know, I’ll say something,

I kind of respond to negativity with negativity.

And then you observe the results of that.

And then over time, you think, wait a minute.

This thing that I’ve been doing where

when somebody says, you suck, and you say, no, you suck.

That never produces the result you thought it might.

And so might not want to just, don’t say you suck back.

And I do this through a lot of things in life.

I’m very fortunate to not suffer from depression,

but first of all, I have had and have people in my life

who do, and also, you know, all of us have depression

who don’t suffer from depression, have depression out.

Like, it’s always knocking on the door.

Right, yeah.

And so you have mild, I mean,

if you’re very careless with the triggers all around you,

then you’re just, I think all of us have the capacity

to really suffer from that kind of chemical

or psychological or philosophical existential crisis.

But then it raises a question, why are we built this way?

It seems like it doesn’t make sense, right?

And here’s where some of us thinking about

where we came from as the human family

is kind of interesting.

It doesn’t make sense that somewhere on that spectrum

that it’s good to detect that there’s an array

of adverse forces out there in the world right now

at this moment and to withdraw, to hunker down,

to not fight, not strive, not try to meet the challenge

and outweigh these negative forces

that are present out there.

And that makes a lot of sense, and all animals

that have been studied in one form or another show this.

Even the worm that I mentioned earlier,

C. elegans with 302 neurons, it can effectively give up

in challenging situations.

We’ve done this with zebrafish,

tiny little transparent fish.

You can give them a challenging situation

and they will give up, but then if you stimulate

a couple very specific brain regions in particular ways,

you can motivate them to overcome the challenge.

And if you inhibit those regions,

they give up much more easily than they would otherwise.

You can do this in mice, you can do this in rats.

So this is an ancestral conserve pattern

to detect that things are pretty bad out there

and to conserve energy, to hunker down,

to wait out the storm.

So as you, unfortunately, many of our maladies

have useful roots that contribute to our survival.

So both depression and motivation have uses.

And sometimes it’s nice to just shut the hell up

and huddle with the penguins versus,

for some unknown reason, venture out on your own

into the mountains like a David Goggins type character.

So what’s the difference to you between,

you see patients, between sort of rigorous psychoanalysis?

I don’t know if you consider talk therapy

and psychoanalysis, are they neighbors,

are they overlapping?

They’re neighbors.

Psychoanalysis is, they’re relatively,

it’s not nearly done as much as the talk therapy,

like the cognitive behavioral therapy I mentioned.

The psychoanalysis is a little more niche now

and partly because it’s not, the data isn’t,

in terms of actual treatment of actual therapeutic effects,

data not as supportive as for cognitive behavioral therapy.

But it’s still interesting as for insight,

people, a lot of people still do it

to gain insight into themselves.

And in general, it’s a good sort of conversation starter.

Those methods, they’re good for getting things out.

We don’t focus on dreams typically these days

in psychiatry, but they’re great conversation starters.

They’re great ways to get things out if people have,

and so we like to use those methods

just to get the ball rolling sometimes,

get people to open up a little bit.

But the actual treatment tends not to involve

these psychoanalytic approaches where you are really

probing the unconscious mind and its manifestation

through dreams, for example, as the goal.

That’s not the goal.

Modern talk therapy, we’re really focusing on treatment,

how to get people to feel better.

See, I use that as a conversation opener,

the Freudian thing where I try to delve at a bar

of the deep sexual desires in a person’s subconscious

and I find that opens up possibilities very quickly.

Now, what’s, I mean, this is a silly sounding question,

but what’s the difference between

cognitive behavioral therapy and conversation?

So, because I personally, as a fan of conversations,

as a fan of just, I like listening to podcasts

versus like audio book, I like both,

but they’re very different and I like conversation.

I like, it makes me personally very anxious,

so I like to be the listener, like a third wheel,

like overhearing a conversation kind of thing,

but it’s a really powerful method for humans

to explore each other’s mind, just raw conversation.

So, do you think it can be more productive

to be very systematic about it or is conversation itself

the art form of helping each other,

understanding each other and helping each other?

There are forms of talk therapy

that are essentially conversational

or they much more approach pure conversation.

There’s a befriending therapy,

there’s interpersonal therapy.

These are approaches that are purely talk therapy,

but they’re not as structured

as cognitive behavioral therapy.

Cognitive behavioral therapy is,

there are manuals, there are guidelines.

You can almost go through it in a very cookbooky way.

There’s homework that you get done.

So, it’s in its fullest form,

it’s very different from these

more conversational strategies.

But what’s interesting is sometimes people compare them

and so you’ll see almost like randomized controlled studies

comparing cognitive behavioral therapy

with interpersonal therapy, for example.

And they both can work and actually in some studies,

they look comparable.

So, to your point, conversation and insights

that come from conversation, if done well,

if done artfully, can be as powerful.

This reminds me of Robin Williams.

I have to ask you several questions here on that.

But one of my favorite movies is Good Will Hunting.

I don’t know if you’ve seen it with Robin Williams.

So, as a psychiatrist yourself,

can you do a deep analysis of this other famous psychiatrist

which is the movie character played by Robin Williams

at Good Will Hunting?

Is it just a caricature between a psychiatrist

and patient relationship?

Or is there something to you that was moving

about his ability to connect

to this obviously struggling young kid?

I think you hit on the key thing there

which is the depth of the connection.

If there’s too powerful a connection,

that can impair therapy

because it could impair open communication.

If someone, if a patient has a, sees the role,

sees the relationship in a particular way,

like in a friendly way maybe,

or like a parental child type way,

that can cause problems because then what they choose

to share, what they choose to bring up is selected

to be appropriate for that view of the relationship.

And so, I and many other talk therapists actually prefer

not to let things get, not let the connection get that deep.

You wanna have trust.

You wanna have a therapeutic alliance, we sometimes call it.

But it’s got to be enough of a blank slate

that the patient is not consciously

or unconsciously constrained in what they choose to share.

And so, great movie, great actors, all good,

no complaints except realistically,

the relationship should be a little more arm’s length

than that.

Let’s pretend this is real life.

Sometimes can’t you leave a little bit of yourself

in the interaction with the patient?

I mean, it’s another human being.

Yes, so there’s a balance.

And actually you do need some of it

because let’s say this person is having challenges,

interpersonal challenges in their life.

The best way to notice what those are

and to identify them and to work with them

is if you can elicit some of those problems

in the office, in the therapeutic interaction.

And this is really powerful.

As long as you’re alert to it, aware of it,

and you don’t let it go out of hand,

this transference, we call it,

is when you transfer in between

the current therapeutic relationship

and external relationships

that the patient may have had with others.

And so if the therapist starts to feel

an inner feeling like anger, let’s say.

So let’s say you have a patient

who is stirring frustration in you

or even in extreme cases, anger,

the best thing for the therapist to do in that case

is to recognize it and to realize

that’s probably being stirred by other people

in the patient’s life.

And that could be the source of a lot of problems.

And so instead of trying to wall it off,

and say, oh, I shouldn’t be feeling that,

I better be a better therapist instead,

and recognize it and use it,

and help the patient that way.

And so you’ve gotta be a human being.

You’ve gotta be a person who feels.

You’ve gotta be open.

But be in control of it and be aware of it.

If I may, I just wanna read,

because it’s one of my favorite scenes.

Probably one of the greatest scenes,

one of the greatest scenes in movie history

because Robin Williams does a single take.

Is that right?

I didn’t know that.

So this is a very interesting interaction between them.

So Will, and I’m sure this is a common interaction,

maybe with a therapist and a patient,

maybe with a father and son,

where Will, the young character,

the young, brilliant mathematician,

and Sean is the therapist, the older therapist,

where Will looks at a painting that Sean painted

and then does a deep, critical analysis of the painting

that basically describes pretending

as if he can understand another human being completely

by just looking at their painting.

And then Sean gives this whole speech

that contrasts sort of raw intelligence

and the wisdom of experience.

And Sean says, single take.

He says, you’ve never been out of Boston, right?

And Will says, nope.

All this in a sexy Boston accent, by the way.

And then Sean gives the speech.

If I asked you about art,

you’d probably give me the skinny

and about every art book ever written.

Michelangelo, you know a lot about him.

Life’s work, political aspirations,

him and the Pope, sexual orientation,

the whole works, right?

But I bet you can’t tell me what it smells like

in the Sistine Chapel.

You never actually stood there

and looked up at that beautiful ceiling, seeing that.

If I asked you about women,

you’ll probably give me a syllabus

of your personal favorites.

You may have even been laid a few times.

The language here is just beautiful.

But you can’t tell me what it feels like

to wake up next to a woman and feel truly happy.

You’re a tough kid.

If I asked you about war,

you’d probably throw Shakespeare at me, right?

Probably not, but let’s say.

Once more into the breach here, friends.

But you’ve never been near one.

You’ve never held your best friend’s head on your lap

and watched him gasp his last breath,

looking to you for help.

If I asked you about love,

you’d probably quote me a sonnet.

But you’ve never looked at a woman

and be truly vulnerable,

known someone who can level you with their eyes,

feeling like God put an angel on earth just for you,

who could rescue you from the depths of hell

and you wouldn’t know what it’s like to be her angel.

To have that love for her,

be there forever, through anything, through cancer.

And you wouldn’t know about sleeping,

sitting up in a hospital room for two months,

holding her hand because the doctors could see in your eyes

the terms visiting hours don’t apply to you.

You don’t know about real loss

because that only occurs when you love something

more than you love yourself.

I doubt you’ve ever dared to love anybody that much.

I look at you.

I don’t see an intelligent, confident man.

I see a cocky, scared, shitless kid.

But you’re a genius, Will.

No one denies that.

No one can possibly understand the depths of you.

But you presume to know everything about me

because you saw a painting of mine

you ripped my fucking life apart.

You’re an orphan, right?

Do you think I know the first thing

about how hard your life has been,

how you feel, who you are because I read Oliver Twist?

Does that encapsulate you?

Personally, I don’t give a shit about all that

because you know what?

I can’t learn anything from you

that I can’t read in some fucking book

unless you want to talk about you, who you are.

And I’m fascinated, I’m in.

But you don’t want to do that, do you, sport?

You’re terrified of what you might say.

Your move, chief.

Well done, sir, I know it’s a movie.

It’s interesting, right?

So some of that conversation

is at some intellectual level, too.

It’s not just emotional, it’s something,

it’s like, the reason I kind of connect with that is

that’s a lot of work for a therapist.

Like, to really understand another,

because he’s, I mean, from, okay, I know this is fictional,

but just, there’s calculation happening.

He deeply cares to say the words

that the other person needs to hear,

but also a little bit loses himself in the pride,

but then catches himself again,

switches from anger to connection.

Yeah.

A lot is brought up there.

You’re right, there has to be some emotion

in the therapist to care enough to keep going,

to keep probing, to open up as he’s doing so, right?

He revealed a lot about himself, his own vulnerabilities,

but that gave him authenticity.

He had to open himself up

so that the kid would see the authenticity

and open himself up in return.

So how do you do that as a psychiatrist, as a therapist?

You have to be careful.

You don’t wanna do too much,

but opening up a little bit does help.

It does create a chance.

You’re offering up something

and that helps the patient come back in return,

and it gives you that believability and authenticity.

Do you pay the price for that, for opening it up?

You can.

You have a family.

You have an incredibly difficult research.

You’re doing a lot of things in your world.

I mean, it’s the price you pay for like.

Well, this was one of the terrifying things

about writing the book was I do open up

in a little bit about my own personal life,

my own personal challenges,

and that was a considered decision

because I could have done the patient work

and the science work

and the history of the human family work

and tied it all together,

but it wasn’t, and in an early draft, it was like that,

but it wasn’t real yet.

It wasn’t something that everybody could connect with,

and I said, then I realized, look, if I’m gonna do this,

I’ve gotta open up myself,

and then people can connect with me

and see what I’m really saying, and so I did,

and that was not something that I’d gone in planning to do.

In retrospect, I learned a lot about myself.

It was actually really, I think, a good thing that I did,

but it was scary.

Where are the darkest places you’ve ever gone in your life?

You know, I had, things haven’t always been easy,

personally or professionally.

I had moments, you know, I was effectively a single dad

for a while, a number of years,

and these came at probably the hardest,

also, professional lifetimes for me, too,

the absolute hardest days of late medical school,

internship, taking call, getting up at 3 a.m.,

surgery, medicine, rounds, unforgiving environments,

and then all the while, personal life,

stripped down to the bare, and these were low moments,

and then I was hit particularly hard by

just experiences on the clinical ward,

connecting too deeply with patients,

like a child with a brain tumor,

and feeling it too strongly, and those things,

when you get down to those lowest of the low moments,

when everything is stripped away,

and there’s only this raw core,

well, that’s pretty hard.

That was probably the lowest moment,

and you learn a lot about yourself in those moments,

you know, what’s left, and then what are the roots out

from there, and that can be powerful to see in yourself.

Have you thought about killing yourself?

I have not.

Have you seen that thought in the distance?

I am fortunate that that has not come to my mind,

and I have not seen it, even in the distance,

and in some ways, I’ve wondered if that’s made me,

am I a less effective psychiatrist because of that?

I’ve been, I’ve felt everything stripped away.

I’ve been at the lowest of the low, and yet, that.

There’s still hope.

There’s a light of hope still at the end of the tunnel.

So you never lost, even for brief moments, that.

Never did.

I don’t know why.

You don’t know why.

There was no reason.

You don’t know why.

No reason to feel hope at that moment, honestly.

Uh, so it was just the light without reason.

Yeah, that’s right.

What wisdom do you draw from that time?

About, so first of all, you said something funny,

which is, I wonder if it, that it’s somehow

not having thoughts of suicide limits your capacity

to truly understand somebody who is having those thoughts.

So how many demons must a psychiatrist have

in order to be a good psychiatrist?

You know, this is a really interesting question.

I think everybody knows, and I can say this,

that psychiatrists can be a little unusual.

We think about ourselves, right?

We think about our brains.

That may be one reason why we become psychiatrists

is we think, oh, that’s interesting going on in there.

What’s that about?

So a little introspective, a little introverted maybe,

and that’s what can make us good when we’re good.

And, but also that may select for people

who have some unusual aspects,

but you don’t have to have all of them.

There’s a lot that can go wrong in the psychiatric realm.

I think having some of those, some of it,

but not all of it is enough.

You get to see how low things can get.

You can get, you get empathy from that,

even if the symptoms are not the same.

Just empathy for struggle, for suffering.

That’s right, that’s right.

Do you yourself have to practice observing triggers

just as a human operating in this world?

I’ve definitely, those skills that have come from therapy,

I’ve found them useful, yeah.

If I noticed that, we’ve all been through experiences

where we wonder, oh, I got really mad in that interaction.

Why did I get that mad?

Yeah, sure, maybe I could have been irritated,

but man, why did I?

And then thinking about it and realizing,

okay, back up here, think about the broader context.

Think about how that relates to prior events in my life.

Okay, yeah, so this is a thing for me

when something of this class happens, then it triggers me.

So going forward, I’m gonna be aware of that.

And I’ve definitely used that

because you don’t wanna be out of control of those emotions.

You wanna identify them.

You wanna know where they come from

and you wanna head them off

as a civilized human being living on this earth,

trying to get along with other people.

You wanna understand those moments.

Let me return to Robin Williams for a second

and looking at Robin Williams, the actor,

sorry, the human,

because you mentioned for depression,

you can have everything going well.

And I think there’s just famous cases of just public figures

because a lot of people know them,

where they suffer quietly

and it seems like from the outside perspective

that they have everything going for them,

that they’re at the top of their career.

Two people that come to mind

are Robin Williams and Anthony Bourdain.

What insight do you have in why either of those have taken,

why Robin Williams, a comedian,

one of sort of the most jolly humans?

Obviously, there was always the darkness

that he was channeling in order to present the happiness,

but it feels like that realness is only possible

when you’re deeply self honest and analytical

and then if you’re deeply self honest,

you’re going to realize that there’s a lot

of beautiful things about life that you can discover

and if you do that,

how can you possibly then take your own life?

I mean, you go through all of these thoughts

and I think a lot of people really loved Robin Williams,

which is why it was really difficult to see

how can even him, how can even Robin Williams

take his own life?

So I don’t know if there’s something to be said

about the nature of depression

from just looking at his case.

I think the action of suicide is not well understood.

It doesn’t always, although often,

is correlated with depression.

There are cases of suicide where there is not

clear depression, that’s in the minority.

By the way, if I just,

because you said it so interesting,

action of suicide, because there’s also thoughts of suicide

and probably those, they’re probably somewhat understood,

but it’s an interesting, because you can think of suicide,

if you have suicidal ideation,

you can think of that for so many reasons.

That’s right.

And I mean, thoughts sometimes, like painful thoughts,

angry thoughts, or thoughts in general,

can be very different, like fantasies, for example.

You can fantasize, like sexual fantasies.

You can fantasize, I was just for humor’s sake

wanted to mention stuff, but then people think I’m serious,

so I’m not gonna mention anything.

But sexual fantasies, and then there’s,

I know there’s people that have sexual fantasies

and they don’t wanna actually do that in real life.

That sexual fantasy serves some kind of purpose

in imagination only, and in that same way,

suicide might serve a purpose in imagination only,

is very unlikely to lead to action.

And yet there’s other thoughts that maybe are more amorphous

that do lead to action, and that leap,

yeah, that, oh boy, that’s a fascinating,

and that’s such a philosophically powerful thought

to not exist, like that question, that’s the,

is it Sarcher or Camus, Camus?

Well, the myth is Sisyphus, Camus, who says,

like basic question of why live?

Good question.

Yeah, right.

So that’s a great question, actually,

and there are other related questions.

Some people may have the thought of suicide

because there seems no point, there’s no joy in life.

That’s one reason that some people can put forward.

Sometimes there’s an, it’s not just the absence of joy,

there’s an active pain, an active psychic pain

in some people, and that, the inescapability of that

is enough to drive the thoughts of suicide.

And then there are interpersonal and cultural reasons

as well that can show up.

But the act, this act of ending of the self is,

in all these cases, there’s no real way to study this

in animals, no other animal as far as we know

that we can study has this concept of this is myself,

the situation is not tolerable, therefore,

I will end the self.

To our knowledge, this is not something

that can be studied in other animals.

So it remains this very poorly understood action.

And in predicting it, so what do we do as psychiatrists?

We have this challenge.

People come to the emergency room, they say they’re suicidal

or their friends say they’re suicidal

or they’ve taken some action that didn’t lead to death.

What do we do?

Well, there’s a whole range of options.

Was it a suicidal gesture in the sense

of not intending death or was the intent death?

And if it was the intent was death,

what were the reasons?

Are the reasons transient?

Are they gone now?

What’s the probability that it’ll be repeated?

So we do all these things just to decide

what sort of treatment should be carried out,

but nowhere is there a deep understanding of the biology,

of the cells and circuits and activity patterns

that underlie the action to end the self.

It’s a very, it’s this frustrating thing.

It’s so timely, it’s so common, it shows up in veterans,

it shows up in kids, it shows up in people

at every stage of life, and yet we’re very bad

at understanding it and we’re relatively poor

at predicting it and our tools are not very powerful.

We can put people in a locked unit,

we can give them care, therapy for a while.

At some point, we release them

and there’s only so much we can do.

It’s one of the most frustrating things,

the suffering that is linked to suicidality.

But it is a decision and it is an action

and if you look at optogenetics,

you should be able to one day sort of understand

the dynamics of such weighty decisions.

The individual causes then, if someone is anhedonic,

if there is no joy in life, that very likely

is addressable by optogenetics.

We know how to turn that dial very robustly in animals.

The motivation to overcome challenges,

that we have some hope of understanding.

Psychic pain, internal negative states,

we have actually a handle on that as well.

There’s a structure in the brain called the habenula

and some linked structures around it

that seems to generate this negative internal state.

It’s active when a state of acute disappointment,

acute outcomes that go wrong, not as expected.

Moments of unexpected pain.

The habenula is there, it seems,

it’s active to report on internal negativity

with its action.

And so you could imagine strategies

to target this brain structure

that might have the effect of reducing psychic pain,

reducing the negativity of internal states.

That is a very concrete hope.

It’s precise, it’s anatomical.

Optogenetics has given us all the firm foundation we need

to go after that question.

So I think there is hope.

If you look at the individual causes,

the individual symptoms relating to suicide,

and then it’s like a puzzle,

you put together the puzzle pieces.

By the way, I do think my habenula is

very functioning, very actively.

And I wonder if it’s like,

because you can also learn to channel these things, right?

Some of the things we suffer from,

I mean, there’s degrees of suffering,

can be a source of progress and personal growth

and development and all those kinds of things.

I mean, I, what is it?

Nietzsche suffered from stomach issues.

I wonder if he’s written some of those things

if his stomach was all great.

I mean, there’s, I kind of think that

a difficult life in some form,

you can get, you get to choose in some regard

and some of you don’t.

The difficulties you have and the ones you do have,

it’s nice to use if possible.

Sometimes it’s nice to treat,

sometimes it’s nice to use.

Well, the way you phrase it, I think you’re using it.

I could be wrong, but if you,

you phrased in this semi humorous way about your habenula,

it seems to me that you’re using that to good effect.

Now, but one never really knows

what someone else’s internal state is.

As I look at you, I don’t know the depths of what’s going on

and it’s possible that it’s a much harder situation

in there.

Yes.

So that’s, I actually worry about this a lot.

So I’m extremely self critical,

like in the privacy of my own mind,

which is an interesting thing

when you get to meet the internet

and the internet will tell you you suck.

But for now, now this is what I worry about

and I’m very paying attention.

For now it’s really, I just have this like very negative

voice, but that voice seems to be very useful

for productivity and so I channel it.

I just put it on the table and let that voice like

talk to me, but I’m very, I’m like monitoring that voice

because looking at Robin Williams, you know,

you get older, your brain changes or like you’re,

and then that voice can now all of a sudden grow, right?

And then where you can’t control as much,

you have to be very careful with these kinds of things.

You’re very right about that.

So my negativity, I have this,

I never think I’ve done enough is sort of where my

negativity comes from inside.

I never think that I’ve met the potential of the moment.

I haven’t done, I haven’t, you know,

made the most of the opportunities that are available.

Still early, I haven’t, you know,

progressed as far as I should.

And exactly as you’re saying, that works for a while.

But then what happens as you get later in life

and there’s less runway to, you know, fix that.

And then maybe then that negative voice is a problem.

But also at that point, the negative voice

starts having more and more of a point.

When you’re being very successful,

it’s easy to be like, no, okay, well,

like because later in life, you’re really literally

just sitting there on a rocking chair doing nothing.

And then it’s, or maybe any kind of tragedy happens.

Loss of a loved one, loss of a job,

loss or you get screwed over in some kind of way.

I don’t know.

And then all of a sudden the negative voice

is just you and the negative voice

for days and days and days.

And so I don’t know, to go back to your example

of Robin Williams, I don’t know what was going on inside him.

I don’t know the nature of his internal state.

Was it active psychic pain that?

May I mention, may I interrupt to just say

that Sergei posted an examination of Robin Williams.

His brain tissue suggested that he suffered from quote,

diffuse LEWY, Lewy body dementia, LBD.

Depression is a symptom of LBD and it’s not about psychology.

It’s rooted in urology.

This is words from Sergei.

His brain was falling apart.

Yeah, Lewy body dementia.

This is a very interesting neurological disorder

where among other things, there’s neuron death indeed.

So you’ve got frank neuron loss.

It’s not just a matter of some longstanding psychic pain,

but you’ve got a progressive loss.

And so clearly you’ve got a situation

where he could have finally reached a point

where the balance that he’d worked out

between negativity and positivity was disrupted due to loss.

The wrong cells died, the wrong projections were cut.

By the Lewy body dementia.

Certainly dopamine neurons die in Lewy body dementia.

Those are neurons that give rise to much of the feelings

of reward and pleasure that we experience among other roles.

So clearly in his case, there could have been

a very concrete cellular neurological issue

that was progressive and pushed him to that point.

But were you about to make a point about broader

that if there is not a neurological degeneration?

Yeah, so in his case, not knowing that,

it could have been simply that,

let’s say he had an internal psychic pain state

and he was in sort of a compensated mode

for much of his life, able to generate enough joy

from his comedy and his social interactions.

But eventually later in life, those things drop away,

the balance shifts.

You get tired of fighting the pain for that long.

So you’ve got this time dependent non stationarity

that happens and then the same symptom

becomes no longer tolerable in the end.

What is autism?

What do we know about autism?

Human beings exist on a spectrum of how social we can be.

And this is pretty interesting actually, scientifically,

but also very important clinically.

There are hyper social states where people

are almost too social.

There are chromosomal deletion states

where people have instant affinity and bonding

and rich deep seeming connections with people, very verbal.

On the other end, people with autism spectrum disorder

are not able to keep up with social interactions

and it’s a spectrum.

Some have mild to moderate difficulties.

They may have inability to understand

what the next thing to do in a social situation is,

but may have perfectly good language abilities.

And as you progress further along the spectrum,

that gets more and more severe

so they can’t make eye contact because it’s too overwhelming

to think about what has to be done next

if a person looks in a particular way.

And then as you go farther,

then language and social communication themselves break down

so there’s no reciprocity, there’s no shared enjoyment.

And that this gets very hard then

as you get to this far end of the spectrum

where there’s really an absence of social cognition at all

and social bonding.

So why does this exist?

What is it?

It’s very genetic.

As I mentioned, it’s one of the top three or four

most biological in the sense of most genetically determined

of the psychiatric illnesses.

It does have these interesting positive correlations,

slight positive correlations

with intelligence and education.

And the reason for that

is kind of interesting to think about.

Is there something good about it?

Just like, or at least with at least part of the spectrum,

is there something good about it?

Just as we were talking about for depression,

as you could say for mania,

as you could say for schizophrenia.

And here it’s kind of interesting

to think about the underlying science

of what it means to be good at a social interaction.

Someone who’s very good at a social interaction

is incredibly good at dealing

with unpredictable information,

is able to handle this torrent of information

coming through rapidly changing

model of the other person and of the interaction

and their model of you, your model of them.

With each word that changes,

with each new bit of information

that comes in through the conversation,

each bit of body language, all this is rapidly changing.

And some people are able to keep up

with that firehose of information perfectly well.

But that’s a special brain state to be in.

That’s working with unpredictability.

That’s the only way that can be done

is most likely by constantly running models

of what the other person might be about to say.

So you can’t stop and think, oh, what did that word mean?

What did that shift in eye contact mean?

What do they mean together?

There has to be some advanced work going on

where you’re predicting what’s going on

if you’re to keep up with a rich

and fast social interaction.

Now, on the flip side, there are brain states

that maybe don’t have to work so fast

but that are extremely important still.

Dealing with something that’s not moving

or that’s predictable, still complex,

like mathematical proof or a very complex arrangement

of geometrical shapes, a large number

of individual nonmoving things.

There’s possibly a way of being that’s particularly good

at dealing with these static, unmoving,

or predictable situations and less so

with these rapidly changing social situations.

And so the way I conceptualize autism

is these are people whose brains are not so good

with the high bit rate, unpredictable information,

but may be quite good at given enough time,

given the grace to work with the system,

to look at it from different angles,

to take different perspectives with a confidence

that it’s not changing in between perspectives.

That’s a brain state that’s valuable.

It’s something that has probably contributed

to a lot of the success of the human family,

being able to design something,

being able to consider all the different contributions

to a static, predictable system.

So autism, in a sense, is a spectrum

that has identifiable characteristics

about the way people deal with dynamic information,

often expressed itself as like social dynamic information.

But you critically, your use of the word often there

is really, I think, smart,

because it’s not just social interaction

that is a challenge in autism.

And so many people conceptualize it purely

as a social dysfunction disorder.

But it’s really any unpredictable information

that’s a problem, that’s a challenge

for people on the spectrum.

They react very negatively to unexpected sounds,

even if not social sounds, unexpected lights,

unexpected touches, and so it’s really

unpredictable information that is, in my view,

the core problem with the processing in autism,

not just social.

Social just shows up because it’s so unpredictable.

Yeah, it’s so interesting.

I mean, I try to not to think about that stuff.

I’m afraid of thinking about disorders

and things like that because just like I don’t like

sort of economics or game theory,

I want to be careful with it because it,

whenever you have a category or a model,

it’s too easy to just, for everything,

I mean, it’s the OCD thing.

I like models too much.

I like categories too much.

The moment you acknowledge yourself,

well, I have an eating disorder, for example,

or something like that, as opposed to just being a,

well, I’ll just leave it at that

from my own critical understanding of myself.

Let’s just say I don’t know how to moderate eating fruit.

People make fun of me.

They think all fruit is healthy.

I know.

I don’t know how to moderate anything,

but even fruit, apples and cherries, is a nightmare.

Anyway, that’s such a psychiatrist thing to say.

Very interesting.

Thank you.

But there’s characteristics and it’s interesting

to think about, like for example,

I have trouble making eye contact,

but I actually, as you said it now,

it’s not that I’m shy at all in that sense.

It’s literally, I’m getting way too much information

and it’s distracting me.

Like I need to just close my eyes so I can,

like all the things that people seem to be able to do

in parallel, it’s just, you just asked me a question.

For me to think about the answer to that question,

I can’t have all this cool, rich visual information

coming my way.

That’s literally, because I often close my eyes to think.

It’s not because I’m afraid of something, whatever.

It’s just like too much information happening here.

Well, that’s a beautiful description.

It’s amazing that that is how you experience

the eye contact aspect.

I think that’s, I mean, you’ve articulated

what captures it for so many people,

which is that it’s overwhelming.

There’s just too much information just coming in

through the eyes and to keep up with it,

to know you’re gonna be expected to keep up with it,

first of all, so there’s that aspect.

You know, you’ve learned socially

that there’s gonna be an expectation

if you’re making eye contact.

People are gonna think you’re keeping up with it,

and you don’t want to because you wanna focus

on other things and make progress in other dimensions.

Yeah, and so then there’s a strong desire

to look away or to close the eyes

because it’s overwhelming, it’s a distraction,

and it’s gonna cause errors of understanding.

And of course, our eyes, that’s part,

the way we use our eyes is part of the human communication,

so you have to kind of be aware of that,

of that element of it.

So yeah, I mean, but it’s fascinating.

You should be aware of your own self

in those little characteristics,

whether it’s classified on some aspect

of the autism spectrum or just in general,

whether it’s eating, whether it’s depression,

whether it’s even like schizophrenia

that I hope we get a chance to talk to a little bit.

Yeah, but those things are all made up

of different symptoms and characteristics,

and use them as a superpower, I suppose,

is the best we can hope for in mild cases, I guess.

I do think both brain states can’t coexist

at the same time.

The way of dealing with something unpredictable

and dealing with something predictable,

those are different ways of being,

here’s a huge opportunity for very creative

model building in theoretical neuroscience

and linking that to these data streams

we’re getting across the brain that we talked about earlier,

these immense data sets of activity across the brain.

Here’s where I think there could be

a real convergence of theoreticians and experimentalists

to say, okay, given what we know about wiring of the brain,

here is what the brain state is likely to be

that deals well with unpredictable information,

and here’s the brain state

that deals with predictable information.

Here’s why they’re incompatible, at least at the same time.

Here’s why you’ve gotta be able to detect

which state you should be in.

Here’s how you could switch between them.

Here’s the kind of cells that you would predict,

almost like predicting the Higgs boson.

Here’s the kind of circuitry that I would predict

should govern the switching,

or might make one state too sticky, too hard to get out of.

That is a huge opportunity for an interaction

from the theoretical and experimental side together.

Make one state too sticky.

The sort of measure the stickiness of the state

and how to lessen the stickiness.

Get some oil in the machine.

Yes, yeah, what would predict the kind of oil

that would work well.

What, in your practice, is treatment or advice

for the people on the autism spectrum?

So right now, there’s no real medical treatment.

There are behavioral treatments

that are most effective early in life.

They make sure people don’t fall too far behind.

If you’re not interacting socially,

you create this vicious cycle

where you fall farther and farther behind

because you’re not interacting.

And these therapies which are applied early in life,

therapists work with the kids,

train them to deal with these things

that otherwise would be aversive to them,

teach them how to predict things and interact,

and that has a big effect.

But it’s behavioral therapy.

There’s no medicine that works.

There are ways of reducing individual symptoms though

that sometimes come along with autism

and those do respond to medications.

So you can, one thing, very often,

my patients with autism are very anxious

because they live in a world

that they have a really hard time

predicting what’s gonna happen.

And so they find, and some of these are high functioning,

Silicon Valley types who they may make great livings

but they’re very unhappy because they’re on the spectrum.

They don’t understand how social interactions really work.

They’re very anxiety provoking

because they don’t know what to say.

They don’t have any clue how anybody else knows what to say.

They’re constantly worried they’re gonna say something

that’s completely inappropriate

and so they’re very anxious.

And I can treat their anxiety.

It doesn’t touch the autism per se

but I can help them with their anxiety.

What I just talked about, eye contact.

I am richly, even with eyes closed

and all those kinds of things,

I’m richly experiencing the world.

And it’s not like you’re afraid of the world

or you’re not able, I don’t know what to do.

No, I know everything.

In fact, I know way too much.

There’s so many cool options.

Like at any one moment, there’s all the stuff happening

and it’s all beautiful.

And at any one moment, you can do anything you want.

You can take off your clothes.

You can punch that guy over there.

You can run away.

You can go in for a hug.

You can say something profound and deep

or you can say something generic

or you can do so many things you can say.

And then it’ll go, it’ll unravel in all these kinds of ways

and this moment could be completely life changing

or it can be mundane and meaningless.

And all of those options are before you at any one moment.

And so it’s like, it’s amazing and overwhelming

if you allow yourself to think about it,

which whatever, exactly.

Like, well, I’m fortunate with chess,

you have a few set options.

Two dimensional, at least dimensional is constraints.

There is unlimited possibilities

and unlimited beautiful things happening all around you.

So I don’t think there’s a kind of sense

that somehow you’re limited in the places of,

in the way you can see the world

and how you can interact with that world.

I am overwhelmed by the lack of limit.

That all of us should be, have you looked around?

You can do whatever the hell you want.

Nobody will remember you anyway.

All of us will be dead one day.

You could do anything.

You can, I don’t know, you can get naked

and run around the city,

as long as you’re not hurting anybody,

and it doesn’t matter.

So it’s Austin, anyway.

Austin, yeah, exactly.

Seems like a to do item for anybody living in Austin,

for sure.

But the spectrum is an interesting concept

because that is, when I say,

when I refer to the spectrum,

I’m actually referring to, it’s a precise clinical term,

but you’re right, it’s been coopted more broadly

and it is widely used and it can be

an unfair categorization of someone

who’s socially and occupationally very healthy.

And that is critical

because we don’t define a disorder

unless there’s social or occupational dysfunction.

It doesn’t matter what the symptoms are.

I’ve had patients who are pleasantly hallucinating,

so frankly, psychotic, but doesn’t affect their lives,

so I don’t give that person a diagnosis

because there’s not social or occupational dysfunction.

Same with anything on this,

any of the diverse symptoms of autism spectrum disorder.

If someone has them,

but they’re successful socially and occupationally,

we don’t say that there’s a disorder.

But then you’re right, that the concept of the spectrum

does become a useful pigeonholing device,

which is maybe not the best thing.

Yeah, and the eye contact is an interesting one,

is an interesting one.

I’m torn on it.

I’m torn about the usefulness of eye contact

because people kind of make fun of it,

but let me just say one thing about eye contact

and about life in general.

It’s okay to be weird,

but like some people, when you have your eyes closed

and there’s that weird, what is happening to this creature?

Like you see a weird creature on the side of the road.

It’s interesting.

And you wanna, I mean, the weird stuff,

I’m gonna go back to Robin Williams with the,

that’s the good stuff, right?

He has that whole speech about him and his wife

and what he loves all the little peculiarities,

all the weird stuff.

And that, like let those flourish.

Let those, like celebrate those in yourself

and not in some kind of woke way,

but in some like very human way.

This is what makes us, this is the weirdness.

Yeah, I’m 100% on board with that.

And I don’t think, you know, people who are happy

and who have people in their lives who are happy with them,

these are, I think, let the weirdness flourish.

Let the, all the different ways members

of the human family can be different.

Let’s see them all.

That’s one of our, that’s one of the joys of being alive

is seeing all the ways we can be human.

And I think about it all the time.

Why do we have all these ways of being human?

And even within one individual,

you go through phases of life

where you express different sides of your way of being,

which is also a pretty fun opportunity, right?

You can go through phases where you’re in one mode

and phases when you’re in another mode.

And let that, you know, just let that flourish too.

Let the ways that you can be you vary as well.

I think that’s important for people to explore.

And I should, like, as if you can address the internet,

but I would like to sort of ask the internet

to celebrate the weirdness of people.

Like, that’s, it’s the Robin Williams,

people call these imperfections,

but they’re not, that’s the good stuff.

For any one individual person,

find the weird stuff and celebrate it,

as opposed to what the internet often does,

which is find the weird stuff and criticize it.

Because when you criticize the weird stuff,

you’re creating conformity, which is another human thing.

But that conformity creates a boring world.

You want the weird, you want the crazy.

That’s what fun is made of.

That’s the foundation of humor

and all of the ways in which we deal

with the suffering in the world,

with the injustices in the world,

is like this like huge variety of weird.

Yeah, I don’t know.

And that’s what, at the depth of psychiatry,

is like you wanna acknowledge the weird,

celebrate the weird, like step around it

to find the particular aspects of weird

that are debilitating, like you said.

They’re somehow negatively affecting your ability

to function in the world,

as opposed to trying to shut it all down.

That’s right.

Well, on that topic, I mean,

I’d love to talk to you about schizophrenia.

What is schizophrenia?

From your research and from your general understanding,

and what is the full landscape of suffering

and wisdom that schizophrenia explores?

Schizophrenia is a state where

there is a break from reality.

And so this can show up, as we call them,

the positive symptoms of schizophrenia.

These include hallucinations,

hearing something or seeing something that’s not there,

usually auditory hallucinations.

Paranoia, people can have complex fears.

Delusions, which we call fixed false beliefs,

people get an extremely unshakable

but completely implausible idea about something.

Sometimes it relates to themself, sometimes to the world.

These we call the positive symptoms,

break from reality as we know it.

Then there are the negative symptoms that come with it,

and these are progressive.

These are flattening of emotion, as we call it,

so starting to express less and less positive emotion,

ending more in a neutral or flat state.

Thought disorder, inability to work with complex patterns

of planning or thinking, so you can’t make plans,

you’d have poor working memory,

you can’t keep track of where you were in a conversation,

in a sequence of actions.

So poor and impaired working with the thoughts of oneself

and then these positive symptoms of break from reality.

Okay, now why do these come together?

What’s the neurobiology of it?

Again, we don’t know.

Schizophrenia, extremely genetically determined.

If you look at the numbers,

could be upwards of 80% genetically determined.

1% of the human population around the world,

it’s universal, okay?

It’s not confined to any one culture,

not even really biased in one culture or another,

about 1% around the world.

And has this progressive quality to it, untreated,

so it’s very interesting.

There’s a break that happens, we call it first break,

when someone experiences their first disruption of reality,

they can have a completely typical life up until that point.

So you might have a, and I’ve seen just heartbreaking cases

of like this in the Stanford emergency room

where a kid has come there,

who’s been extremely high functioning in that sense

of academic achievement and athletic and interpersonal,

and then comes to college.

Usually in men, it’s around 18, 19 when the first break

happens, some terrifying paranoia hits

or some auditory hallucinations start.

They’re getting screamed at by a voice in their head.

So devastating.

With women, comes on also often a little later,

sometimes in the 20s, and it can be progressive.

If it’s not treated, it just progresses and progresses.

The voices become overwhelming, the delusions

and paranoia extend and expand.

The thought, the negative symptoms particularly

become more and more severe.

So one can’t even maintain thoughts

in any sort of ordered fashion.

And then eventually, it can be fatal,

it can lead to suicide, it can lead to erratic behavior

that leads to accidents.

Now, it can be treated.

There are medications that help, fortunately.

They have side effects, so they’re not perfect.

You can have movement problems and actually a whole host

of different side effects that come from the medications.

But we can help people now with schizophrenia

very, very significantly.

But the amazing thing, and this is emblematic

of where psychiatry stands, we don’t have

the deep understanding, just like with depression,

we don’t have that heart as a pump level of understanding

that we’d like to have with schizophrenia,

despite it being so biological, so genetic in its nature.

So is there a way to return to the other side

of the first break?

So when you have a break with reality,

is there a way to kind of stitch it together?

So some people, that works, but we don’t really know how.

So medications, antipsychotic medications, we call them,

they block a particular neurotransmitter receptor

called the serotonin 2A receptor,

and they modulate dopamine as well

and other neurotransmitters.

These can take someone who’s actively hallucinating,

actively paranoid, put them back in a completely normal

state, and some people stay that way indefinitely.

So you can bring people back from that,

back to the other side, have it stitched together.

More typically, you’ll end up in some intermediate state

where symptoms are reduced powerfully,

but there might be still something there

and you’ve got a drop down in functioning

that may be persistent for a while.

But concepts, what physically is going on?

One idea is that it’s communication within the brain.

One part of the brain is not able to tell

other parts of the brain what it’s doing.

And so the auditory hallucinations

are very interesting in this regard.

They often have this conversational,

inner monologue like quality.

As we’re walking along the street,

we may have an inner monologue,

thoughts about what’s going on.

If we see somebody we don’t like,

we may have a thought,

wish somebody would punch that guy, something like that,

or maybe I should punch that guy.

But these are so far below where we would ever act

or even think of acting,

but they’re just things that come up.

And in people with schizophrenia,

those inner thoughts, that inner monologue,

is not recognized as the inner monologue of the self.

And so it’s perceived as something coming from the outside

or from inside, but from another entity.

Another, oh, another, I thought you meant

like another room inside the same building.

Another room inside there, yeah.

And so that’s, so it could be conceptualized

as a communication within the brain problem,

notifying another part of the brain what’s going on.

And there’s some evidence consistent with that.

I don’t know if you can help with this,

but I sometimes, so I’ve been talking

to quite a few homeless folks recently,

just, so what I do is I hang out at night

and talk to interesting people.

And some of them, and I’ve known people in the past

who suffer from schizophrenia,

and some of them, like self, will describe

as that as something they suffer from.

And they seem to understand something deeply

about this world.

I don’t know if it’s correlated

or maybe it’s another aspect of like depression,

all those things that I’ve encountered in my own life

is maybe just the struggle and the suffering

has taken you through a life

where you think deeply about life.

Like there’s like self reflection

that society forces on you

because it’s a disorder of some kind.

It’s interesting, I guess my only sort

of anecdotal observation is people who suffer

from schizophrenia seem to be very interesting

and very thoughtful in a nonlinear way about the world.

I’ve noticed that it’s not always positive.

There are unusual ways they view the world.

It was, but it’s always interesting.

That could be conspiratorial thinking too.

Like, but like the theories they have

about the way the world functions,

often very well read, which is also interesting

because they’re almost like looking

for helpful answers from somewhere.

Absolutely there.

And so they’re, they might be citing

some very interesting literature

and then using that to, there’s a stickiness

in their mind to different models of the world

and trying to make sense of that world.

And those models could include conspiracy theories.

Yeah, they’re very attuned to complexity

and they come up with unlikely explanations,

which is one of the things that makes them,

it makes it hard for them to function in the world

is how unlikely their explanations are.

But you’re right, there’s a depth of consideration

of the complexity of the world and a concern about it

and a work, an impulse to work to understand it

that is actually quite refreshing.

But the first case in the medical literature,

there was a classical schizophrenia.

There was a patient named James Tilly Matthews

who had this, he sketched out for his doctor

the experiences he was sensing

and he drew himself as a cowering figure

on the ground controlled by a loom,

a weaving device that was sending threads,

long threads, projections across space

from the loom to him, to his arms and to his body

and controlling him from afar.

And he called this the air loom, a loom in the air.

And it was such an evocative thing

because this was the start of the Industrial Revolution

or mid and it was where really industrial strength,

looms and weaving devices were really kind of the emblematic

of the most complex, powerful technological achievements

of the time and so that was the explanation available

to him to explain how his body was seemingly moved

without his volition and these days, of course,

people with schizophrenia will have more technology

appropriate interpretations, they’ll have delusions

of satellite or alien control or beamed information,

very, very common to have this delusion

of a government agency sending electromagnetic

or radio frequency information to control their limbs.

But it’s the same thing, whether it’s a thread

from an Industrial Revolution loom

or RF radiation, it’s the same thing just adapted

to the moment explaining, trying to explain

the world they live in and their relationship to the world.

But unconstrained by sort of the thing

that’s socially acceptable,

which is both refreshing and dangerous.

Yes.

I wrote down a question.

Why do we cry?

Are tears a window to some depths

that we ourselves don’t know?

I almost wanna make fun of myself for that question,

but you do talk seriously about crying in the book.

In fact, the whole first chapter really tussles

with crying as why do we do it, what does it mean,

why is it involuntary?

It seems like a weakness, right?

Because it’s so involuntary and it’s reflecting

something true and inside.

At the level of the individual,

that seems like a problem, right?

Wouldn’t it be better if we could control it,

if we could not show that emotion when it’s not useful,

show it when it’s useful?

But it’s not, it’s largely involuntary.

And so there’s a value to it, I think,

as an honest reporter of a need,

of hope and frailty at the same time.

I am a human being, there’s a frailty to myself

or my situation where I need social help,

I need help from my community.

I have hope that that is possible,

but I’m not enough for myself, I need the community.

That I think is what the social signal of crying is.

Now people have studied crying, it’s an extreme,

you can quantify the extent to which the presence of tears

on a face triggers reactions in onlookers.

And you can show the same face in the presence

or absence of tears and show that to people

under quantifiable and rigorous psychological conditions.

And tears are much more powerful at stirring

the desire to help in viewers

than any other facial feature.

Which is pretty interesting that it’s the honest one

that’s also the most powerful, right?

It kind of indicates there’s a certain logic

to our design as social beings

that we have an honest report.

That’s hard to control.

But is it well understood how that connects

to the internal state of emotion?

Yeah, there are long range projections that come,

so where is crying generated?

This is the confusing thing about it.

So that we have a little tear duct, the lacrimal gland

that leads to the release of fluid,

it ejects fluid and it comes out.

And those of course, that whole system was designed

to keep the eye clean, to wash out particulate irritants.

So it’s a long standing, as long as we’ve had eyes

and have been out of the water in our evolution,

we’ve needed this sort of thing.

So long standing biological structure,

recently co opted it seems by our evolution

as social primates.

Now, how could that happen?

Well, the lacrimal gland is controlled by structures

in the pons, which is a structure deep in our,

just above our neck, between our neck and our head.

And reflecting its ancient origin, right?

As you go farther down toward the spinal cord,

these are the more basic early evolved structures.

And in the pons, that’s where breathing is controlled,

tear duct contraction.

And what we found and with optogenetics,

we helped sort this out,

there are long range projections

from fear and anxiety regions in the forebrain

that project all the way to the pons

in and around those areas.

The reason those are there,

we think is to regulate the respiratory rate changes,

the breathing changes of fear and anxiety.

So we know when we’re in a state of fear and anxiety,

we need, we cope better if we have elevated heart rate,

elevated respiratory rate, more blood pumping around,

more oxygenated blood,

we’re ready to meet the threat if it happens.

All those cells are down there in the pons too,

right next to the lacrimal duct, the tear gland neurons.

And so almost certainly this fear anxiety induced crying

arose from a very slightly misdirected long range projection

that was there to regulate breathing.

And a little twist, just a little misdirection,

a little missing of one sign post to stop here,

going on a little farther,

getting to the lacrimal gland neurons gave us crying.

And that’s, and we just have it,

that peculiar sort of structure, neuronal structure

that resulted in that, that’s what we’re stuck with.

And that ends up being, in terms of social interaction,

one of the more important, authentic,

involuntary displays of interstate.

That’s right.

And social communication.

Yeah.

Oh, yeah, is there other stuff like that?

I mean, do you, yeah, I mean, the human face is fascinating

as a display of emotion, as a display of truth and lying

and all those kinds of things.

I personally, I mean, we’re all, I suppose,

have different sensors that are sensitive

to certain aspects of the human face.

But to me, it seems like the eyes

are really important communication or something.

You know, I’ve talked to a few sort of girls

about like Botox and stuff like that.

And it always bothers me when,

I guess guys can do this too,

but like when women speak negatively

of, I guess you can call them wrinkles,

at the tips of an eye.

But like to me, when you smile, when you wink,

not wink, but like narrow the eyes,

something is communicated and those,

that stuff is really useful, the human face.

And when it’s gone, something is missing.

And a lot of little stuff, it feels like it really,

it’s almost involuntary, I guess,

but it’s harder to describe as the presence

or absence of tears.

It’s like something about this person,

you can tell they’re not bullshitting you.

Yeah, yeah.

And so that was what made, presumably,

that tear recruitment so powerful,

is it just landed in this very high value real estate

for social communication.

If it had gone to, you know,

there’s a lot of neurons in the pons

that control movement of large muscles elsewhere,

that would have been much less effective

as a social signal than something around the eye.

So it was, however that little misdirection happened,

it landed in a great area for social communication.

And because it was coming from the fear and anxiety circuits

that regulate that necessary involuntary change

in heart rate and respiratory rate,

it also was involuntary and that became valuable

as a truth signal, as social beings.

So very interesting when you think about

the origins of the human family,

the origins of social structures

and our ability and need to call for help

when there’s hope, but need at the same time.

What is consciousness, Carl?

So you’re actually using techniques.

I mean, even putting psychiatry aside,

just looking at optogenetics,

you’re trying to understand some of these deep aspects

of the human mind.

And maybe this is a good time to return to a question

you mentioned you might have an opinion on

if there’s such a thing as a theory of everything

for the human mind.

Because surely answering of what is consciousness

is as, well, that’s not sure.

But it seems like it’s a fundamental part

of the human experience in the human mind

and solving that question will result in solving

the bigger thing about the human mind.

The flip side could be consciousness

is just the few neurons that are generating

some useful thing that make us,

it’s like the sense of self

that you talked about in the mice.

Maybe it’s a subset of those cells

that are just creating a richer sense of self

and that’s it.

So this is a great question.

All neuroscientists think about this

and a lot of non neuroscientists too.

It’s the reason a lot of people came to the study

of the brain is to think about consciousness

and not just being awake or alert

but really what’s sometimes called

the hard problem of consciousness

which is what is that nature of that inner

subjective sense we have?

Not just information processing

but feeling something about the information.

What is that inner state of subjectivity physically?

What is it?

And that’s called the hard problem of consciousness

and it’s not a extremely well defined question.

Everybody has sort of a sense of what it means

but it’s such a hard problem

because you run into paradoxes quite quickly

the more you think about it

and that is exciting also

because it makes us think

actually there’s some fundamental,

there’s a big thing that we’re missing.

The brain is not just a collection of little tricks.

There is a big, big concept.

So that’s your sense of the big

because a flip side could be with optogenetics.

You can, there’s an engineering question.

Can you turn consciousness on and off like a light switch?

Okay, so here’s where exactly consciousness

frames the problem extremely well

and it frames it the following way.

So I told you earlier that we can stimulate

20 or 25 cells in the visual cortex of a mouse

and we can make it behave and we can make its brain act

as if it’s seeing something that isn’t there.

We have that level of control now.

We can pick out 25 neurons, play in activity

and both behavior and in the brain,

it’s as if it’s seeing something specific.

Okay, now let’s do a thought experiment,

you know, a Gedanken experiment and let’s play this out.

Let’s say we could do the same thing

for every single neuron in the brain of a human being.

Let’s say we had total control

and I could do something like,

I could show you a rich deep color red

and you could look at it and you would be aware

that it’s red, but also you might have some feelings

about it, something would be stirred in you,

some subjective sense as you looked at that rich color red

and then I would take away the visual stimulus

and I would, in this thought experiment,

I would, using some hyperoptogenetics,

I would play in exactly the same pattern of activity

in every cell in your brain for as long as was needed,

whatever, 15 seconds, something like that,

that exactly matched what was going on

when you were feeling that inner subjective sense.

Okay, so in that thought experiment,

a question for you is would you be feeling

that same inner subjective sense?

Stimulus is gone, every neuron’s doing the same thing

because I’m controlling it.

There’s a philosophical question there.

If you ask me specifically, I would say yes.

Okay, good, most people would say that

because it’s hard to say no, right?

It’s very hard to say no.

If every cell in your brain is doing what it was doing,

what else could be different?

How could? Well, most normal people would say yes.

Of course, philosophers would then start saying no.

They’re the ones that say,

I’m in sort of parallel and sorry

if it’s a bit of an interruption,

but if there’s a robot that’s conscious in front of you,

if it appears conscious, then it’s conscious.

Like to me, of course, philosophers again speak up

and say, well, no, how do you know it’s conscious?

Well, how do you know anything is conscious?

And sort of as normal humans,

we tend to lean on the experience

versus some kind of philosophical concept.

So the great thing about what you just said,

the Turing test is it’s very practical.

If it acts conscious, it is conscious.

But I think that’s limiting.

I like the thought experiment.

I think it’s actually more informative.

And so I’m halfway to the conclusion there,

but let’s take it as your answer was yes,

that you would be feeling the same thing.

Okay, now here’s where it gets fun.

Now that every cell in your brain knows what it has to do

in the sense that we know it and we’re providing it,

your brain cells don’t need to be in your head

anymore at all, right?

The only reason they’re next to each other,

the only reason they’re wired together

is to affect each other, to stimulate or inhibit each other.

But we don’t need that anymore

because optogenetically,

we’re providing that activity pattern

for as long as needed.

We’re providing the effect of the communication.

They don’t need to be connected anymore.

They don’t even need to be in your head.

I could spread your neurons all over the continent,

all over the galaxy,

and I could still provide the same stimulus pattern

over 10 or 15 seconds to all those neurons.

And somewhere Lex Friedman would have to be,

even though no longer existing as a physical object anymore,

would be feeling that subjective feeling.

And it’s inescapable

because it’s exactly the same as the previous situation.

All the neurons have to be spatially,

like the locality constraint,

they have to be spatially close to each other.

And you talk about light opto,

which is funny because light is the fastest traveling thing

that we know of.

Maybe let’s not put them all over the universe

because we might get relativistic problems then.

Let’s just keep them out.

Let’s keep all your neurons.

Let’s spread them over North America, okay?

And let’s play them out, same pattern of activity.

And right, it seems absurd, right?

There’s no way that could be true.

There’s no way that Lex would be feeling

that internal sense

if his neurons were spread all over North America.

And yet it’s exactly the same as the previous situation

where you said, sure.

So we’ve got a paradox.

And this is what makes people think.

Is it a paradox though, sorry?

Well, maybe paradox is the wrong word.

We got a problem.

We got a problem because it reveals

that there’s something big about those,

that internal subjective state that we’re not explaining.

And we don’t really have a hope of explaining

in the near future.

But don’t you think we would still have that?

It’s just the word internal loses meaning,

but don’t you think we still have

that internal subjective state?

Or if not, then where the heck is the magic coming from?

Okay, well, I just think,

I think one of the problems

that I think we need to let go of

is we tend to, outside of the experience of consciousness,

the hard problem of consciousness,

we tend to think that we individual humans

are really special.

Not the subjective experience,

but the entirety of it, like the body

that contains the thing.

So the local, the constraint of all the stuff

has to be together, and it’s all mine.

That’s a very, I don’t know if that has anything to do

with the mechanisms that are creating this.

So in fact, one really nice way to break through that

is to either observe or create consciousness

that spans multiple organisms.

Sort of like, let’s say it’s not an organism dependent

phenomena, that the phenomena can,

that’s just a peculiar way it has evolved on Earth,

but it’s a phenomena that doesn’t have anything to do

with a specific biological system.

Right, so and we have different parts of our brain exist

and sometimes create complex awarenesses of things

that involve different neurons that are distributed widely

and that need to communicate with each other

to form this joint representation,

this state of consciousness.

But indeed, why do they have to be in the same head?

We don’t know why that would be the case that they do.

And so that’s a huge unanswered question in the field

is what is it that binds the activity of neurons together

so they can form a joint representation?

And actually this comes back to the dissociation experiment

we talked about before, where your sense of self

becomes separated from your body.

Those things that were fused in a joint representation,

the same concept, unitary, are now separate.

And in late 2020, we published a paper in Nature

showing how this could be.

We used optogenetics to drive this rhythm

that ketamine and PCP cause in retrosplenial cortex

and we got different parts of the brain to be out of sync

and when they were active, never able to be active

at the same time, never able to form

a joint representation at the same time.

And so we’ve got a toehold into these questions.

We don’t have the answers, but.

And that mimics the dynamics of ketamine effects.

Exactly, exactly.

And you’re able to find that kind of oscillation.

Wow, wow, wow.

I see if you get even greater and greater control

with more control over individual neurons

and understanding, like if you think of certain neurons

that having some role to play in the sense of self,

you can play like an orchestra.

That, to create certain degrees of consciousness,

degrees of subjectivity, and thereby understand

what is consciousness.

But by having a very complicated light switch essentially.

And here’s the challenge is the nice thing

about the thought experiment is it kind of highlights

that we’re gonna hit a point where

we’re addressing some very, very fundamental questions.

What allows the activity of two sets of neurons

to become mutually relevant to each other?

This is in some ways maybe one of the deepest

remaining questions in neuroscience is what allows

activity patterns to become relevant to each other?

Do they have to be in sync temporarily?

Do they need to be, is there some other quality

that we don’t know about that also needs to be present

to allow cells to fuse together into a joint representation?

Just so I understand, because it feels close

to some very, very deep idea.

So there’s a bunch of semi distributed signals

going on in the brain.

And you’re saying there could be something

like a theory of everything if one to exist

is to understand why, how and why signals

close to each other start becoming relevant to each other

as part of some very much bigger signal

that they’re producing.

How they coordinate, essentially.

Because it’s very distributed.

I mean that’s a kind of, within a distributed system,

how is order achieved?

And this is a very specific kind of distributed system

that is one of the most intelligent that we’re aware of

in the known universe.

In that would maybe be something also,

an understanding of the full conscious experience too.

That this kind of coordination.

How does the coordination between different neurons

that are responsible for sense of self,

how do they begin to form a big picture

that we see as a human experience?

That’s really, really interesting.

So uniting the small and the,

I mean that’s actually literally theory of everything.

Uniting the small, the sort of the theory of the neuron.

The functioning of the neuron with the big.

Just the functioning of the entire mind.

That’s right.

And I think keeping a toehold in both

at the cellular level of resolution

and the brain wide resolution will be critical.

If you lose touch with either,

I think you’ll miss the big insight.

So that’s what we’re trying to do.

Keeping grounded in the cellular resolution.

Trying to keep the broadest brain wide perspective

and meet in the middle.

Do you think you’ll see it in your lifetime?

A major breakthrough in that dimension.

I have hope.

It’s very hard to predict what will happen

with big things like this.

If we don’t get there,

there’ll be plenty of other exciting stuff.

So it’s okay.

But the other aspect of this whole thing

is that your life is pretty short.

Yeah, that’s true.

So first of all, you can die any day.

I tend to try to think about that,

that it ends, it can end any moment

because it really, really can.

And if not, it’ll be soon anyway.

Do you think about that?

Do you think about your mortality?

I do, yeah.

It comes back to what we talked about earlier.

I never think I’ve done enough

and it’s relevant to that for sure.

There’s a deadline.

Do you think there’s ever going to be a feeling

where you sit back and you’re really proud of yourself?

I hope so.

Like, I’ve done enough.

I’ve done everything there is.

Because the thing is,

a warrior has some number of battles in them.

And at a certain point, if you’re deeply honest,

it’s like, well, that was a pretty good run.

As far as runs goes, that was pretty good.

And you can hang up your helmet

and then go sort of drink some ale,

listen to some music with the old lady,

and say, I did pretty good.

You think you’ll get there?

You know, with something,

nature always has surprises for us.

The curious mind is always after more.

But biology gives us other rewards.

Children and family, community.

And one can feel good about those things.

Biology is full of rewards.

But do you think about those rewards?

What do you think is the why of those rewards?

What’s the meaning of life and this existence?

What’s the why of biology?

What does it want from us?

Why are all these cells very busy

putting together an organism

that seems to want to just be in a hurry to do stuff

and survive, but it’s not happy being survived.

Like you said, it’s curious.

It keeps wanting to get into more trouble.

Why?

Yeah, you know, we’re clearly designed for that, right?

We’re clearly designed to ask why and to answer.

And that, I think, is,

I don’t know the meaning of all life.

I think a meaning of our lives is that.

And this is the Aristotelian happiness.

An organism is happy, an animal’s happy

if it’s performing to its design, right?

If it’s doing what it was made for.

Yeah, well, you have to understand, what’s the design?

And, you know, who is the designer

and what were they up to and how hard is it?

Do you have to build the whole universe?

And does the design even know what the hell they’re doing?

Because, you know, maybe the designer built humans

to find out about themselves.

That’s what I would do.

Like, if I had the power to build clones,

I would build a lot of clones

and I would get them into different trouble

to understand, like, what am I designed,

what’s this body designed to do?

How far can I go that way?

Exactly.

And then, and I dissociate myself completely

from having any way to know, like, that I know that person.

Oh, that’s good.

I mean, I suppose you could do that

in a single person’s body, but dissociation, you could.

Yeah, but I do wonder what,

if you look at Earth as a collection of humans,

as a collection of biological organisms,

it seems that we’re busy doing something

and it just seems too beautiful and too special

to be a random, a random experiment.

It seems like it’s an experiment that’s cleverly designed

That’s right.

by some forces of nature

that are beyond our current understanding.

And maybe that’s part of our design,

is to keep asking why.

You said answer.

I’m not sure that’s part of the design, the answer.

I think we’re given just the sufficiently limited

cognitive capability that we know how to long

to find the answer and we lack the ability

to find the answer.

That’s basically a summary of your career.

No, I’m just kidding.

And then we give each other Nobel Prizes

for having even an inkling of a good step

towards the right direction.

Carl, you’re an incredible human being.

I’m a huge fan of who you are as a person,

who you are as a scientist, who you are as a writer.

I just thank you so much.

I’m so honored that you would sit down

and talk to me today.

It was amazing.

It’s been incredibly fun.

Let’s do it again sometime.

Let’s do it again.

It’s been really great.

Your insights and wit and modesty

are really quite rewarding.

Thank you so much, man.

Thanks for listening to this conversation

with Carl Diceroth.

To support this podcast,

please check out our sponsors in the description.

And now, let me leave you with some words from Carl Jung.

Knowing your own darkness is the best method

for dealing with the darkness of other people.

Thank you for listening and hope to see you next time.

comments powered by Disqus